Feature Article Heads Together: A Peer-Mediated Option for Improving the Academic Achievement of Heterogeneous Learning Groups Larry Maheady, Barbara Mallette, Gregory F. Harper, and Katherine Sacca The present study compared the effects of two teacher questioning strategies, Heads Together and Whole Group, on elementary school students' on-task rates and their performance on daily social studies quizzes. Twenty 3rd graders in a racially and ethnically integrated classroom were exposed to both treatments under an alternating treatment design. Data indicated that students always performed better under Heads Together conditions. These performance differences ranged from 2 to 30 percentage points on daily quizzes and averaged about 16 percentage points over the course of the investigation. Moreover, the percentage of pupils on task was always higher when the cooperative learning intervention was in effect. Consumer satisfaction findings suggest that the students themselves preferred Heads Together over the more traditional teaching method. Implications for instructional practice are discussed. T HE 1 9 9 0 S P O S E unique instructional challenges for all educators. Rapidly c h a n g i n g d e m o g r a p h i c s , e x hortations for m o r e integrated programming, and an exponential increase in the k n o w l e d g e base to b e mastered have placed teachers in the unenviable position o f having to d o m o r e with less and ultimately calling it "better." Meeting such challenges will require legislative, administrative, and, most importantly, instructional technologies that will allow us to educate students with wideranging abilities in the regular c l a s s r o o m . T h e characteristics o f this instructional t e c h n o l o g y are n o t entirely clear, n o r are the p r o c e d u r e s , m o d e l s , and/or delivery systems for carrying out increased integration fully d e v e l o p e d and validated ( G r e e n w o o d , Maheady, & Carta, in press). Nonetheless, several m e t h o d s have b e e n e m p l o y e d recently to a c c o m m o d a t e h e t e r o g e n e o u s groups o f students, including those with mild disabilities, in general education settings. S u c h m e t h o d s m a y b e categorized into at least four general domains: (a) teacherdirected strategies (e.g., direct instruction, mastery learning, precision teaching), (b) peer-mediated interventions (e.g., c o o p e r a t i v e learning, reciprocal teaching, p e e r tutoring), (c) student-managed m e t h o d s (e.g., learning strategy curriculum, self-instructional and self-managem e n t systems, Personalized S y s t e m o f Instruction), and (d) computer-assisted instruction (e.g., interactive videodisc, a c a d e m i c grading and m o n i t o r i n g systems). Here, w e will d e s c r i b e a c l a s s r o o m intervention that c o m b i n e s e l e m e n t s o f teacher-directed and peer-mediated instruction. Heads T o g e t h e r (Kagan, 1 9 8 9 - 9 0 ) is a teacher questioning strategy designed t o actively engage all students during adult-led instruction and discussion. Since early G r e e k times, t e a c h e r q u e s t i o n i n g has b e e n c o n s i d e r e d very important to student learning. T h e e x act role that q u e s t i o n i n g plays in learning, h o w e v e r , remains unclear (Golden, Gersten, & W o o d w a r d , 1990). It has b e e n suggested, for e x a m p l e , that frequent teacher questioning (a) activates pupils' b a c k g r o u n d knowledge, (b) p r o m o t e s interest in lessons, (c) challenges students intellectually with higher order queries, and (d) improves student c o m p r e h e n s i o n o f lecture a n d discussion material (see, for e x a m p l e , Anderson, Evertson, & B r o p h y , 1 9 7 9 ; Gersten, Carnine, & Williams, 1 9 8 1 ; W o o l f o l k , 1989). If this w e r e true, then the ideal questioning format w o u l d b e o n e that actively involves all students simultaneously. In that w a y , e v e r y o n e w o u l d have an equal Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 opportunity to b e involved and r e s p o n d to teacherdirected queries. Unfortunately, this is not possible with "traditional" c l a s s r o o m instruction. B r o p h y and Everts o n ( 1 9 7 6 ) n o t e d , for e x a m p l e , that the m o s t c o m m o n teacher questioning strategy involves directing questions to the entire class and then calling o n volunteers to respond. Typically, this tactic results in m o r e frequent responding from high achieving students and less active i n v o l v e m e n t from their l o w achieving peers. T h e issue b e c o m e s , therefore, h o w t o turn this situation around, that is, h o w to increase active participation among lower performing pupils w h i l e maintaining the i n v o l v e m e n t o f higher achieving students. Heads T o g e t h e r offers o n e alternative. Heads T o g e t h e r (HT) w o r k s in the following way. First, students are placed into small (four-member), h e t e r o g e n e o u s learning teams consisting o f o n e high achieving, t w o average achieving, and o n e l o w achieving pupil(s). Students n u m b e r themselves 1 t o 4 and sit together during teacher-directed lessons. After the teacher directs a question to the entire class, pupils are instructed t o "put their heads together, c o m e up with their best answer, and make sure that e v e r y o n e o n the team k n o w s the answer." T h e teacher then asks, " H o w many Number ( 1 , 2 , 3, o r 4 ) k n o w the a n s w e r ? " After o n e rand o m l y s e l e c t e d student responds, the t e a c h e r c a n ask, "How many other Number agree with that a n s w e r ? " or, " C a n any N u m b e r expand upon the a n s w e r ? " T e a c h e r s then recognize and/or reward all students w h o provide o r agree with c o r r e c t answers, as well as t h o s e w h o offer meaningful e x p a n s i o n s . S i n c e students are given time to discuss possible answers prior to responding, it is m o r e likely that e v e r y o n e , including l o w achievers, will k n o w the c o r r e c t responses. Moreover, since teams c a n n o t predict w h i c h group m e m b e r will b e called u p o n to respond, t h e y are m o r e likely t o ensure that e v e r y o n e k n o w s the a n s w e r (for a m o r e c o m p l e t e discussion, see Kagan, 1 9 8 9 - 9 0 ) . T o our knowledge, n o experimental studies have b e e n c o n d u c t e d o n Heads T o g e t h e r . T h e purpose o f this study was t o e x a m i n e empirically what effects Heads T o g e t h e r has o n the a c a d e m i c p e r f o r m a n c e o f e l e m e n tary-age students. M o r e specifically, w e c o m p a r e d the effects o f Heads Together (HT) and a W h o l e G r o u p ( W G ) questioning strategy o n third graders' (a) daily social studies quiz p e r f o r m a n c e and (b) on-task rates. In addition, w e examined h o w these t w o interventions affected a teacher's questioning b e h a v i o r and pupils' responses during class sessions. Method Subjects and Setting T w e n t y students ( 1 0 males and 1 0 females) w h o w e r e e n r o l l e d in a general third-grade c l a s s r o o m in a racially and ethnically integrated s c h o o l s y s t e m participated in this study. T h e student population consisted o f 11 ( 5 5 % ) Caucasian, 6 ( 3 0 % ) Hispanic, and 3 ( 1 5 % ) African American children from low i n c o m e to middle i n c o m e h o m e s . A p p r o x i m a t e l y 4 0 % o f the target students w e r e receiving supplementary services in e i t h e r the district's remedial o r special e d u c a t i o n p r o g r a m s (i.e., s e v e n Chapter 1 and o n e special education). Six students had previously b e e n retained within grade level. T h e m e a n c h r o n o l o g i c a l age o f the participants was 1 1 1 . 3 4 m o n t h s (range = 1 0 3 t o 1 2 3 m o s . ) . T h e i r m o s t recent achievement test, the Stanford Achievement Test, administered in May 1 9 8 9 , revealed the following grade equivalent s c o r e s : Total Reading X = 3 . 2 3 (range: 1.7 t o 6.6); Total Listening X = 2A7 (range: 1.0 t o 4 . 6 ) ; and Total Environment X = 2 . 7 5 (range: K to 9.0). T w o facets o f these data are important. First, s i n c e almost o n e third o f the students had b e e n retained at least o n c e in grade level, a c h i e v e m e n t results (particularly reading) d o n o t appear t o b e extraordinarily l o w in c o m p a r i s o n to grade level e x p e c t a t i o n s . C o m p a r i s o n s t o c h r o n o l o g i c a l age, h o w e v e r , d o suggest b e l o w average a c h i e v e m e n t levels. S e c o n d , it is significant t o n o t e t h e e x t r e m e variability in student performance, particularly o n the Environment subtest, a m e a s u r e o f students' understanding o f social studies information. S u c h h e t e r o g e n e i t y offers a unique instructional challenge for a w h o l e class intervention such as Heads T o g e t h e r . T h e c l a s s r o o m t e a c h e r was a white female with 15 years o f t e a c h i n g e x p e r i e n c e . S h e had n o formal training in cooperative learning but in the past had used other peer-mediated instructional procedures. A student teacher (a 32-year-old male) also participated during the final phase o f t h e study. It is important t o n o t e that the classr o o m teacher considered this particular class to b e among the most disruptive in h e r teaching career. T h e principal had b e e n called in o n n u m e r o u s o c c a s i o n s t o reprimand the g r o u p for their disruptive behavior. Target Behaviors and Materials T h e present investigation c o l l e c t e d b o t h p r o c e s s and product data o n teacher and students' performance. Process data w e r e gathered using t h e Q u e s t i o n i n g Event R e c o r d i n g F o r m , an e x p e r i m e n t e r - d e v e l o p e d , direct o b s e r v a t i o n system. T h i s s y s t e m r e c o r d e d data o n four facets o f t e a c h e r q u e s t i o n i n g a n d pupil r e s p o n d i n g b e havior. First, observers r e c o r d e d the n u m b e r o f teacherdirected, content-related q u e s t i o n s that o c c u r r e d during 3 0 - m i n u t e social studies lessons. Content-related questions w e r e defined as b e i n g any statements and/or Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 queries directed toward the class that related directly to the content being discussed. Procedural questions, such as " W h o n e e d s a pencil, paper, b o o k , e t c . ? " o r " W h a t d o y o u do i f you k n o w an a n s w e r ? " w e r e n o t recorded. After e a c h q u e s t i o n was tallied, o b s e r v e r s also n o t e d (a) t h e n u m b e r o f students w h o raised their h a n d s to respond* (b) the a c c u r a c y o r inaccuracy o f e a c h response, a n d (c) the teacher's r e s p o n s e t o e a c h student's answer. T h e teacher's responses w e r e c o d e d with a ( + ) if she acknowledged, praised, and/or gave points for a c o r r e c t answer; if the teacher ignored and/or reprimanded a student following a r e s p o n s e , o b s e r v e r s r e c o r d e d a ( - ) . Finally, if the teacher called u p o n a n o t h e r student t o res p o n d to and/or e x p a n d u p o n a pupil's answer, t h e n o b s e r v e r s m a r k e d t h e data form w i t h a (c). After three p r e d e t e r m i n e d questions, o b s e r v e r s c o n ducted visual sweeps o f the classroom and r e c o r d e d the n u m b e r o f students w h o w e r e o n task at that particular time. On-task behaviors w e r e defined as b e i n g any instances o f student b e h a v i o r that are appropriate for assigned activities. Appropriate behaviors include (a) eyes directed toward the t e a c h e r while s h e is speaking, (b) students reading and/or writing in response to a teacher directive, (c) students discussing content-related material, and (d) students raising hands to r e s p o n d t o questions. Any pupils w h o w e r e off task (i.e., making noise, exhibiting inappropriate m o t o r m o v e m e n t s , and/or staring away from the lesson focus) w e r e n o t c o u n t e d . At the e n d o f e a c h o b s e r v a t i o n session, o b s e r v e r s c o m p l e t e d procedural implementation checklists. Procedural checklists w e r e d e v e l o p e d to measure the accuracy with which the independent variables (i.e., Heads Together a n d W h o l e Group) w e r e implemented. W h o l e G r o u p fidelity checklists listed n i n e steps i n v o l v e d in carrying out this instructional p r o c e d u r e (e.g., teacher asks content-related questions, students raise hands to respond, and t e a c h e r calls o n individual student to answer questions). Observers c h e c k e d each step as either present or absent during the t e a c h i n g session. Fidelity or a c c u r a c y o f i m p l e m e n t a t i o n was then calculated b y dividing the n u m b e r o f steps present b y t h e n u m b e r present and absent, and then multiplying b y 1 0 0 . T h e Heads T o g e t h e r checklist c o n s i s t e d o f 18 procedural steps that w e r e s c o r e d in an identical m a n n e r . Dependent Variable T h e primary d e p e n d e n t variable in this investigation was the percentage c o r r e c t o n 10-item quizzes administered at t h e e n d o f e a c h social studies lesson. T h e quizzes w e r e d e v e l o p e d collaboratively b y the first author and classroom t e a c h e r using a three-step process. First, the t e a c h e r highlighted critical information from the existing social studies text, Regions and Social Needs in Communities, that w o u l d b e c o v e r e d during e a c h class session. T h e e x p e r i m e n t e r then generated sets o f 10 factual k n o w l e d g e questions that assessed students' understanding o f this c o n t e n t . Q u e s t i o n s typically required fill-in-the-blank and short answer, written res p o n s e s from students. T h e t e a c h e r then r e v i e w e d the lists o f questions, making additions, deletions, and modi- fications w h e r e v e r necessary. T o p i c s c o v e r e d o v e r the course o f the study included (a) geographic and historical information a b o u t W a s h i n g t o n , D C , (b) p e o p l e and mountains o f the w o r l d (i.e., t h e Rockies, Alps, and Himalayas), and (c) p e o p l e , c u s t o m s , a n d g e o g r a p h y o f Puerto Rico. S i x t e e n quizzes w e r e d e v e l o p e d prior t o beginning the study, w h i l e the remaining 12 w e r e c o n structed b e f o r e the third e x p e r i m e n t a l phase started. A 20-item consumer satisfaction survey, Heads Together Evaluation Inventory, was d e v e l o p e d and administered orally t o students o n t h e final day o f the study. T h i s survey assessed students' general feelings a b o u t using Heads T o g e t h e r , the acceptability o f specific procedural c o m p o n e n t s , a n d students' satisfaction with a c a d e m i c and social o u t c o m e s associated w i t h its use. O n e final item asked pupils to state their preferences for using either the Heads T o g e t h e r o r W h o l e G r o u p questioning format for the remainder o f t h e s c h o o l year. Data Collection and Reliability O n e graduate assistant and an undergraduate education m a j o r s e r v e d as o b s e r v e r s . B o t h o b s e r v e r s w e r e trained b y the s e c o n d author. O b s e r v e r s w e r e initially provided with a six-page training manual that described p r o p e r etiquette in t h e s c h o o l s and operational definitions o f target b e h a v i o r s . After reading the manual, o b s e r v e r s participated in t w o 1-hour training sessions. Here, they practiced c o d i n g videotaped samples o f social studies instruction in t h e target classroom until interrater reliability levels e x c e e d e d . 8 0 for three c o n s e c u t i v e trials o n all categories o f t h e Q u e s t i o n i n g Event R e c o r d i n g Form. Interrater agreement was s c o r e d i f both observers: (a) c h e c k e d an equal n u m b e r o f questions asked, (b) concurred o n the accuracy/inaccuracy o f students' responses, (c) agreed within o n e o n t h e n u m b e r o f pupils w h o raised their hands, and (d) c o n c u r r e d within t w o o n the n u m b e r o f students w h o w e r e o n task during visual s w e e p s . Mean p e r c e n t a g e o f a g r e e m e n t for o c c u r r e n c e o f question asking w a s . 9 9 (range = . 9 8 to 1.00); . 9 4 (range = . 8 8 to 1.00) for n u m b e r o f students raising Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 hands; .92 (range = .82 to 1.00) for accuracy/inaccuracy o f students' responses; and .82 (range = .67 to . 9 6 ) for the percentage o f pupils o n task. Interrater reliability c h e c k s w e r e also c o n d u c t e d during 2 5 % o f the experimental sessions b y having b o t h observers c o d e teacher and pupil behavior independently. Mean percentage o f a g r e e m e n t for e a c h o b s e r v e d category ranged from .73 (pupils o n task) to 1.00 (numb e r o f questions asked), with an overall interrater agreem e n t level o f . 9 0 across all categories. O b s e r v e r s w e r e kept blind to the purpose o f the study and w e r e told only that w e were interested in examining teacher questioning patterns. Reliability data w e r e also collected o n students' weekly quiz s c o r e s . T h e first author and c l a s s r o o m t e a c h e r independently s c o r e d 2 5 % o f students' social studies quizzes across all phases o f the investigation. Reliability was calculated using the Kappa statistic (Siegel & Castellan, 1988). Mean percentage o f agreement was K = . 9 0 , with a range o f . 6 0 to 1.00. Finally, interrater reliability was calculated o n o b servers' fidelity o f implementation ratings. During 2 5 % o f the observation sessions, b o t h observers c o m p l e t e d fidelity checklists independently. Agreement levels w e r e also derived using the Kappa statistic. Interrater reliability ranged from . 7 6 to 1.00, with m e a n agreement levels o f AT = . 9 4 for W h o l e G r o u p a n d AT = . 9 7 for Heads Together. Experimental Design and Procedures An alternating treatment design (Barlow & Hayes, 1 9 7 9 ; T a w n e y & Gast, 1 9 8 4 ) was used to c o m p a r e the effectiveness o f the Heads T o g e t h e r and W h o l e G r o u p questioning strategies. T h e basic feature o f this design is the rapid alteration o f two different interventions with an individual or group o f learners. Experimental c o n trol is demonstrated w h e n levels o f student performance vary for e a c h individual treatment ( C o o p e r , 1 9 8 1 ) . According to B a r l o w and Hayes ( 1 9 7 9 ) : " F e w w o u l d question the internal validity o f the alternating treatment design o r the ability o f the design to rule out rival hypotheses. In fact, the testing o f t w o treatments in the same subject within the same time period produces o n e o f the m o s t elegant controls for threats to internal validity" (p. 2 0 3 ) . Baseline. T h e study began immediately after o b servers reached predetermined reliability levels. During this initial phase, the teacher was asked to c o n d u c t social studies lessons using " n o r m a l " instructional routines. T e a c h i n g sessions lasted approximately 3 0 minutes per day and w e r e c o n d u c t e d four times per w e e k . Primary instructional m e t h o d s included lecture and discussion, use o f maps and charts, and a W h o l e G r o u p questioning format. T h e teacher directed questions to the entire class, and students w e r e required to raise their hands to respond. Students used lecture notes and information written o n the c h a l k b o a r d to a n s w e r questions. T e x t b o o k assignments were given o n relevant material before and after individual instructional sessions. N o attempt was m a d e to c o n t r o l t h e n u m b e r o f questions asked o r the c o n t e n t o f t h o s e q u e s t i o n s during class meetings. T h e o n l y adaptation to n o r m a l routine was t h e administration o f a 10-item quiz following e a c h social studies lesson. Quiz questions w e r e read aloud b y the t e a c h e r and students w r o t e their responses independently. Quizzes w e r e s c o r e d and r e t u r n e d the following day. Alternating Treatments. During the s e c o n d phase, students w e r e e x p o s e d to b o t h t h e W h o l e G r o u p ( W G ) and Heads T o g e t h e r (HT) questioning formats within the same day. T o a c c o m m o d a t e design features, the teacher held social studies lessons twice a day, 2 days per w e e k . Intervention order was varied r a n d o m l y b y having the teacher flip a c o i n o n e a c h treatment day. Sessions w e r e held in b o t h the m o r n i n g and afternoon and lasted approximately 3 0 minutes. Quizzes w e r e given immediately after e a c h lesson using the same procedures described under baseline c o n d i t i o n s . Intervention sessions began, b y having the teacher ann o u n c e w h i c h treatment was in effect (i.e., W h o l e Group vs. Heads Together). Students w e r e then required to verbalize the treatment c o n d i t i o n in unison. During W h o l e G r o u p sessions, the t e a c h e r d i r e c t e d k n o w l e d g e - b a s e d questions t o the entire class. Students w e r e required to raise their hands to r e s p o n d , a n d the t e a c h e r s e l e c t e d volunteers at r a n d o m . C o r r e c t answers w e r e a c k n o w l edged and/or praised, w h i l e i n c o r r e c t r e s p o n s e s w e r e c o r r e c t e d b y either t h e t e a c h e r o r o t h e r students. During H T c o n d i t i o n s , students sat in small, heterogeneous learning groups. Learning groups w e r e initially formed using p r o c e d u r e s outlined b y Slavin ( 1 9 8 3 ) . Students w e r e ranked from highest to lowest in achievement, based upon mean quiz scores during baseline. T h e highest achieving pupil was then assigned to T e a m 1, the s e c o n d highest to T e a m 2 , the third highest to T e a m 3, and s o o n until five teams o f four students e a c h w e r e formed. All five teams had at least o n e high, t w o average, and o n e l o w achieving student. Students within each team w e r e t h e n n u m b e r e d from 1 to 4 . T e a m m e m b e r ship r e m a i n e d the s a m e t h r o u g h o u t the investigation. T h e t e a c h e r c o n t i n u e d to direct questions to the entire class during social studies lessons. F o l l o w i n g e a c h question, h o w e v e r , t h e t e a c h e r told all teams to "put your heads together, c o m e up with the best answer y o u can, and m a k e sure e v e r y b o d y o n your team k n o w s the a n s w e r . " T h e teacher waited approximately 2 0 s e c o n d s for students to discuss questions a m o n g themselves. No specific strategies w e r e p r o v i d e d for discussing questions, achieving consensus, offering explanations, and/or ensuring that every group m e m b e r participated. Following Heads T o g e t h e r time, the t e a c h e r said, "All Number ( 1 , 2 , 3, o r 4s) w h o k n o w t h e answer, raise your h a n d s . " O n e volunteer was then s e l e c t e d r a n d o m l y to Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 respond. After e a c h student r e s p o n d e d , the teacher asked, " H o w many other Number agree with that a n s w e r ? " I f student answers w e r e c o r r e c t , the teacher distributed tokens w o r t h 10 points to e a c h team that either (a) provided the c o r r e c t answer or (b) agreed with it. I n c o r r e c t responses and/or disagreements with correct answers r e c e i v e d n o points. At the e n d o f e a c h H T session, students t o o k quizzes independently, following p r o c e d u r e s identical to W h o l e G r o u p sessions. Student quizzes w e r e s c o r e d and returned the following day. Individual quiz s c o r e s w e r e also p o s t e d o n a laminated s c o r e b o a r d in the c l a s s r o o m . T e a m s c o r e s w e r e calculated b y adding individual quiz results and points earned for answering H T questions during social studies lessons. Cumulative team totals w e r e derived b y adding points from b o t h H T sessions e a c h w e e k . R e c o g n i t i o n certificates w e r e then issued to the "Fantastic First," " S u p e r S e c o n d , " a n d "Terrific T h i r d " teams. Students o n e a c h recognized team signed their n a m e s o n the certificates. Certificates w e r e t h e n p o s t e d o n the walls outside the classroom. Each w e e k , a n e w c o m p e t i t i o n began, thereb y ensuring that e v e r y o n e h a d an o p p o r t u n i t y t o b e o n a winning team. Phase 3. In the final phase, t h e m o r e effective intervention, Heads T o g e t h e r , was i m p l e m e n t e d during b o t h m o r n i n g and afternoon social studies lessons. Quizzes w e r e administered i n d e p e n d e n t l y using procedures identical t o previous e x p e r i m e n t a l phases. sions yielded quiz averages ranging from 8 0 % t o 9 7 . 3 6 % (X = 8 6 . 5 5 % ) . T h e m e a n p e r c e n t a g e c o r r e c t for all H T sessions was 8 7 . 0 5 . T h e s e c o n d d e p e n d e n t m e a s u r e was the p e r c e n t a g e o f students w h o w e r e o n task during social studies classes. As s e e n in Figure 2 , o n l y a b o u t half o f the class was o n task during baseline c o n d i t i o n s . W h e n alternating treatments w e r e i n t r o d u c e d , the m e a n percentage o f students o n task fell to an average o f . 3 9 (range = . 2 7 % t o . 5 3 % ) during W h o l e G r o u p sessions, while it rose t o a m e a n o f .71 (range = . 5 0 % t o . 8 3 % ) w h e n Heads T o g e t h e r was in effect. In the final phase, the p e r c e n t a g e o f students o n task d r o p p e d t o slightly o v e r half the class during b o t h m o r n i n g and afternoon HT sessions. On-task rates w e r e l o w e s t o n Days 12 and 13, w h e n the student teacher assumed responsibility for HT implementation. Observational data w e r e also c o l l e c t e d o n p r o c e s s variables. T h e s e data are summarized in T a b l e 1. Pro- Baseline 100^ - Heads 60 — During Phase 3, data w e r e plotted separately b y time o f day. As s e e n in Figure 1, students' m e a n quiz s c o r e s remained high during b o t h m o r n i n g and afternoon sessions. Quiz averages ranged from 7 7 . 3 3 % t o 9 6 . 8 4 % (X = 8 7 . 5 5 % ) for m o r n i n g classes, w h i l e afternoon ses- / V / 80 — - T h e primary d e p e n d e n t variable was the percentage c o r r e c t o n daily social studies quizzes. Data for e a c h phase o f the investigation are presented in Figure 1. During baseline, students' m e a n a c c u r a c y levels o n daily quizzes ranged from 5 7 . 7 7 % to 8 2 . 1 0 % , with a m e a n o f 7 0 . 4 6 % . Approximately o n e third o f the class (seven students) had failing averages ( b e l o w 6 5 % ) , while t w o pupils e a r n e d m e a n s c o r e s a b o v e 9 0 % (A). During the s e c o n d phase, class averages w e r e plotted separately for Heads T o g e t h e r and W h o l e G r o u p c o n d i t i o n s . A visual analysis o f t h e data reveals that students always performed better u n d e r H T c o n d i t i o n s . T h e s e perform a n c e differences ranged from 2 to 3 0 percentage, points and averaged 1 5 . 7 9 % throughout the s e c o n d phase (i.e., W h o l e G r o u p X = 6 8 . 5 0 vs. Heads T o g e t h e r X = 8 4 . 2 9 ) . It is significant that n o student had a failing average under HT conditions, and six pupils maintained averages a b o v e 9 0 % . In contrast, w h e n the W h o l e G r o u p format was used, six students had failing averages and o n l y o n e child maintained an average e x c e e d i n g 9 0 % . Heads Together „ Together 90 — 70— Results Alternating Treatments n \ \ • \n \ A / \ A / Whole V Group \/ V/ o—o a.m. •—• p.m. ' DAYS Figure 1. Shows the mean percentage correct on daily social studies quizzes under Whole Group and Heads Together conditions. Baseline Alternating Treatments Heads Together Heads Together o—o a.m. •—• p.m. i—i—i—i—|—i—I—i—i—| DAYS 15 20 Figure 2. Represents the mean percentage of students who were on task during Whole Group and Heads Together interventions. Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 cedural reliability ratings for i m p l e m e n t a t i o n o f the independent variables r e m a i n e d high across all phases o f this investigation. W h o l e G r o u p treatment was implem e n t e d with 1 0 0 % accuracy across the first t w o experimental phases, while Heads T o g e t h e r fidelity ratings averaged . 9 5 % (range = . 8 9 % to 1 . 0 0 % ) during the t w o final intervention c o n d i t i o n s . T h e s e data suggest that students w e r e indeed presented with t w o distinct treatments and that these treatments w e r e implemented with a high degree o f a c c u r a c y . Regarding teacher questioning patterns, an average o f 13 content-related queries w e r e d i r e c t e d to the entire class during initial baseline conditions. Questioning rates c h a n g e d s o m e w h a t w h e n alternating treatments w e r e introduced. T h e t e a c h e r asked a p p r o x i m a t e l y t w i c e as many questions during W h o l e G r o u p (X = 1 7 . 8 3 ) as opp o s e d to Heads T o g e t h e r ( X = 8 . 8 3 ) c o n d i t i o n s . In the final phase, just under 10 q u e s t i o n s w e r e asked o n average during m o r n i n g a n d afternoon HT sessions. In general, students r e s p o n d e d c o r r e c t l y to m o s t teacherdirected questions. Under W h o l e G r o u p questioning, students r e s p o n d e d c o r r e c t l y 8 5 % and 6 5 % o f the time during the first t w o e x p e r i m e n t a l phases, respectively. Approximately 8 2 % o f the students' responses were correct w h e n Heads T o g e t h e r was used initially. During Phase 3, h o w e v e r , almost all student responses during H T w e r e c o r r e c t . Finally, the c l a s s r o o m t e a c h e r remained highly responsive to students' answers under b o t h e x p e r i m e n t a l c o n d i t i o n s . T a b l e 1 s h o w s the percentage o f c o r r e c t student r e s p o n s e s that w e r e recognized and/or r e w a r d e d b y the c l a s s r o o m teacher. S i n c e the teacher rarely ignored student responses o r called o n others to respond, these data are n o t reported. Social validity data w e r e c o l l e c t e d o n Heads T o g e t h e r immediately following t h e last treatment session. Student r e s p o n s e s are d e p i c t e d in T a b l e 2 . In general, students rated Heads T o g e t h e r quite favorably. Approximately t w o thirds o f t h e class thought that Heads T o g e t h e r s h o u l d definitely b e c o n d u c t e d in s c h o o l , and that t h e y w o u l d use it i f t h e y w e r e teachers. Sixty-eight p e r c e n t o f the students felt that Heads T o g e t h e r treated t h e m very nicely, and all but o n e pupil r e p o r t e d that Heads T o g e t h e r was n o t harmful at all. F u r t h e r m o r e , a b o u t 9 0 % o f the class felt that Heads T o g e t h e r h e l p e d t h e m t o learn social studies c o n t e n t better. Regarding specific p r o c e d u r a l c o m p o n e n t s , approximately 8 5 % o f the students liked b e i n g o n a team, a n d about 9 5 % e n j o y e d earning points, sharing answers with their t e a m m a t e s , and s e e i n g their points o n the s c o r e board. Interestingly, s o m e students also n o t e d social benefits a s s o c i a t e d w i t h t h e use o f Heads T o g e t h e r . F o r e x a m p l e , almost t w o thirds o f t h e class felt that Heads Together helped them b e c o m e better friends with others, while a comparable n u m b e r reported that others thought they w e r e smarter. Overall, 9 0 % o f the class preferred Heads T o g e t h e r o v e r a n s w e r i n g their o w n q u e s t i o n s . It should b e noted, h o w e v e r , that student evaluations w e r e not uniformly positive. Five pupils n o t e d , for e x a m p l e , that Heads T o g e t h e r c o u l d b e " v e r y unfair," while three others r e p o r t e d not liking the i n t e r v e n t i o n at all. Discussion T h e present findings indicate that Heads T o g e t h e r was clearly m o r e effective than W h o l e G r o u p q u e s t i o n i n g Table 1. Process Variables Associated with the Implementation of Alternating Treatments Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 in improving student p e r f o r m a n c e o n daily social studies quizzes. Students' mean quiz scores during Heads T o g e t h e r w e r e approximately 1 6 p e r c e n t a g e points Table 2. Mean Proportion of Students' Responses to the Heads Together Evaluation Inventory (N = 19) higher than under traditional teaching conditions. Student failure o n content-related e x a m s was virtually eliminated w h e n the teacher used Heads T o g e t h e r . T h e magnitude and c o n s i s t e n c y o f students' a c a d e m i c gains takes o n a d d e d i m p o r t a n c e w h e n o n e c o n s i d e r s the substantial variability that existed in pupil performance at the outset. Initially, student a c h i e v e m e n t levels o n standardized measures varied b y as m u c h as 10 years. Y e t , e v e r y o n e in the class benefited under Heads Together. Classroomb a s e d interventions for the 1 9 9 0 s a n d b e y o n d will enc o u n t e r similar a c a d e m i c diversity in the e l e m e n t a r y schools. T h e current results suggest that Heads T o g e t h e r m a y b e o n e instructional o p t i o n for a c c o m m o d a t i n g such variability. T h e findings regarding on-task p e r c e n t a g e s w e r e less impressive. While m o r e students w e r e o n task under H T than during W h o l e G r o u p questioning, the p e r c e n t a g e o f attentive pupils r e m a i n e d at an educationally unacceptable level. Just o v e r half o f the class was o n task during r a n d o m observations—clearly n o t e n o u g h to reflect fully active student i n v o l v e m e n t . O n e possible explanation for such l o w on-task rates may b e that a flaw existed in o u r observational system. F o r e x a m p l e , to determine the p e r c e n t a g e o f students o n task, o b s e r v e r s w e r e instructed to c o u n t pupils after marking each category o n the teacher questioning form. B y this time, students had already r e s p o n d e d to t e a c h e r q u e s t i o n s and m a n y w e r e " c e l e b r a t i n g " their t o k e n rewards. As instructed, o b servers r e c o r d e d students w h o talked as b e i n g o f f task. Had observational s w e e p s b e e n c o n d u c t e d immediately after the t e a c h e r a s k e d questions (while students had their heads together), t h e percentage o f students o n task m a y have b e e n higher. In a n y event, a m o r e careful analysis o f pupils' on-task rates is clearly warranted. It is also important to n o t e the e x t r e m e l y l o w pupil on-task rates during days w h e n t h e student teacher taught lessons (i.e., Days 12 and 13). It was evident from observational data and a n e c d o t a l c o m m e n t s from the teacher and observers that the student teacher had s o m e difficulty managing the class. Students b e c a m e quite e x cited during Heads T o g e t h e r , and o c c a s i o n a l l y it was hard to get t h e m back o n task. This suggests that specific rules and r e i n f o r c e m e n t p r o c e d u r e s m a y b e n e e d e d t o c o n t r o l student " e n t h u s i a s m " during H T sessions. Student responses t o the Heads T o g e t h e r Evaluation Inventory indicated that, for the m o s t part, students enj o y e d this peer-mediated p r o c e d u r e . In fact, o v e r 9 0 % o f the class preferred Heads T o g e t h e r o v e r W h o l e Group questioning. T h e c l a s s r o o m t e a c h e r also n o t e d that Heads T o g e t h e r was an effective and efficient classroom intervention, and that she w o u l d use it again. T h e finding that H T was n o t preferred unanimously, h o w e v e r , raises s o m e unique ethical c o n c e r n s . F o r e x a m p l e , what should teachers d o if s o m e students c h o o s e n o t to w o r k c o o p e r a t i v e l y with their peers? Certainly, t h e y c a n n o t force children to cooperate; yet, c o n d o n i n g student nonc o m p l i a n c e may inadvertently reinforce their unwillingness t o w o r k with others. Future empirical investigations Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 must address c o n c e r n s about the use o f an effective intervention that s o m e students find to b e socially unacceptable. Demonstrating that a particular c l a s s r o o m intervention is effective is o n e thing; explaining " w h y " it w o r k s is another. Heads T o g e t h e r c o n t a i n s n u m e r o u s p r o c e dural c o m p o n e n t s (e.g., structured teaching p r o c e d u r e , interdependent reward contingencies, public posting o f student p e r f o r m a n c e ) that have a l o n g history o f e m pirical support in the behavioral literature. In the present case, it s e e m s reasonable t o assume that student engagement increased w h e n student teams w e r e asked to collaborate o n teacher-directed questions. Students w h o normally w o u l d b e inattentive o r passive during teacher lectures may have "tuned i n " because their peers w e r e discussing instructional c o n t e n t . In addition, because students c o u l d not predict w h o w o u l d b e called u p o n , they m a y have w o r k e d harder t o ensure that e v e r y o n e , including l o w achievers, k n e w t h e c o r r e c t answer. Finally, since t o k e n rewards and team recognition were delivered contingent u p o n peers' collaborative efforts, such student behaviors m a y have b e e n strengthe n e d over the c o u r s e o f the investigation. Each explanation is tentative at b e s t but remains highly consistent with theoretical interpretations from o t h e r c o o p e r a t i v e learning and group c o n t i n g e n c y m e t h o d s (see, for example, G r e e n w o o d & Hops, 1 9 8 1 ; J o h n s o n , J o h n s o n , & Maruyama, 1 9 8 3 ; Kagan, 1 9 8 9 - 9 0 ; Slavin, 1 9 8 3 , 1990). Clearly, m u c h m o r e study is required o n t h e effects o f Heads Together. T o begin with, the present investigation must b e replicated with o t h e r student populations, in different c o n t e n t areas, and certainly for longer periods o f time. It might also b e useful t o use different experimental designs (e.g., ABAB and/or multiple baseline). W h i l e the alternating treatment design used h e r e c o n t r o l s well for threats t o internal validity, s o m e questions remain about the external validity o f this particular design (cf. B a r l o w & Hayes, 1 9 7 9 ; Hains & Baer, 1 9 8 9 ; T a w n e y & Gast, 1 9 8 4 ) . B a r l o w a n d Hayes ( 1 9 7 9 ) noted, for e x a m p l e , that b e c a u s e there are very few applied situations w h e r e i n t w o or m o r e interventions are alternated o n a daily basis, the generalizability o f treatment effects remains uncertain. Additional replications with m o r e "natural" design features, h o w e v e r , should strengthen the external validity o f the present findings. A n u m b e r o f o t h e r c o n c e p t u a l and procedural issues remain regarding the use o f Heads Together. Conceptually, o n e may e x a m i n e the relationship that exists b e t w e e n answering factual recall questions and the acquisition o f so-called " h i g h e r o r d e r " cognitive skills. D o e s Heads Together facilitate or impede higher order conceptualizations o f critical lesson c o n t e n t ? In what ways d o e s it d o so? Similarly, what effect does the use o f Heads T o g e t h e r have upon students' long-term retention and application o f instructional c o n t e n t ? D o students learn and retain information better because they have discussed it and/or explained it t o their peers? Will t h e y apply s u c h "shari n g " strategies in o t h e r subject areas as well? Procedurally, future investigators should d e t e r m i n e when Heads T o g e t h e r " f i t s " b e s t into lessons. S h o u l d teacher-directed questions b e interspersed intermittently throughout lessons, t o maintain e n g a g e m e n t , o r s h o u l d the p r o c e d u r e b e used primarily at the b e g i n n i n g and ending o f lectures, to activate prior k n o w l e d g e and synthesize content-related information? Similarly, it w o u l d b e quite valuable to c o n d u c t direct observations o f pupil interactions during Heads T o g e t h e r time. In particular, it w o u l d b e important t o analyze the c o g n i t i v e " l e v e l " o f c o n t e n t discussions a n d the various roles that e a c h team m e m b e r plays in p r o m o t i n g group c o n s e n s u s . Additional research might also e x a m i n e what effect specific procedural adaptations have o n the overall effectiveness o f Heads T o g e t h e r . F o r e x a m p l e , w e a d d e d t o k e n rewards, w e e k l y team c o m p e t i t i o n , daily quizzes, a n d public p o s t i n g o f students' p e r f o r m a n c e t o the original description o f Heads T o g e t h e r . W o u l d the intervention b e equally effective w i t h o u t s u c h procedural adaptations? A c o m p o n e n t analysis o f this modified peermediated intervention s h o u l d p r o v i d e useful information in this regard. In sum, the present investigation d o c u m e n t e d the effectiveness o f a relatively simple questioning t e c h n i q u e . This p r o c e d u r e required o n l y m i n o r changes in h o w the classroom teacher asked students t o respond t o contentrelated q u e s t i o n s during lessons. T h e present findings provide an initial data b a s e o n the efficacy o f this cooperative learning strategy. M u c h m o r e w o r k must still b e d o n e with Heads T o g e t h e r before w e c a n e x t e n d our results to o t h e r students, settings, o r c o n t e n t areas. T h e r e is s o l a c e in k n o w i n g , h o w e v e r , that the c l a s s r o o m s o f the 1 9 9 0 s will provide us with ample o p p o r t u n i t y t o evaluate o u r efforts. ^ L a r r y M a h e a d y , PhD, is an associate professor in the Department of Education, State University of New York—College at Fredonia. For the past 10 years, he has been involved in the identification and validation of classroom-based instructional strategies that enhance the academic and social performance of mainstreamed learners. Dr. Maheady's current interests include peer-mediated instruction, student-managed instructional options, and developing support systems to assist teachers in carrying out these instructional strategies. Barbara M a U e t t e , PhD, is an assistant professor in the Department of Education, State University of New York—College at Fredonia. A former special education classroom teacher, her current focus is on both peer-mediated instruction and the development of communicative competence among children with disabilities. She is the immediate past president of the New York State Council for Exceptional Children. G r e g o r y F . H a r p e r , PhD, is a professor in the Department of Education, State University of New York—College at Fredonia. A researcher in the Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015 area of peer-mediated instruction, his current research focuses on the use of peer-mediated strategies with children with handicaps to promote academic achievement, and as a means to facilitate integration into mainstream settings. K a t h e r i n e S a c c a , EdD, is currently project coordinator for the Collaborative Teacher Preparation Project, a 5-year, federally funded grant located at SUNY, College at Buffalo. This project involves training preservice teachers to accommodate the needs of atrisk students, difficult-to-teach students, and/or students with handicaps within general education settings. Dr. Sacca also works with numerous school districts in New York State by providing inservice training and implementation assistance in cooperative learning and other peer-mediated instructional approaches. Address: Larry Maheady, Department of Education, State University of New York, College at Fredonia, Fredonia, NY 14063- Authors' Note The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of the admiriistration and faculty o f the Dunkirk Public Schools, Dunkirk, New York. In particular, we wish to thank Ms. Terry Wolfenden, Superintendent o f Schools, Mr. Louis Maheady, Principal, and Ms. Bethany Gugino, third-grade teacher. In addition, we wish to recognize the invaluable assistance o f our observers, Ms. Margaret Dempsey and Ms. Jennifer Kaleta. References Anderson, L.M., Evertson, CM., & Brophy, J.E. (1979). An experimental study o f effective teaching in first-grade reading groups. Elementary School journal, 79, 1 9 3 - 2 2 2 . Barlow, D.H., & Hayes, S.C. (1979). Alternating treatments design: One strategy for comparing the effects o f two treatments in a single subject. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 1 9 9 - 2 1 0 . Brophy, J.E., & Evertson, CM. (1976). Learning from teaching: A developmental approach. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Cooper, J.O. (1981). Measuring behavior (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill. Gersten, R.M., Carnine, D.W., & Williams, P.B. (1981). Measuring implementation o f a structured educational model in an urban school district. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 4, 56-65. Golden, N., Gersten, R., & Woodward, J. (1990). Effectiveness of guided practice during remedial reading instruction: An application o f computer-managed instruction. Elementary School Journal, 90, 2 9 1 - 3 0 4 . Greenwood, C.R., & Hops, H. (1981). Group contingencies and peer behavior change. In P.S. Strain (Ed.), The utilization of peers as behavior change agents (pp. 189-259). New York: Plenum Press. Greenwood, C.R., Maheady, L., & Carta, J.J. (in press). Peer tutoring programs in the regular classroom. In G. Stoner, M.R. Shinn, & H. Walker (Eds.), Interventions for achievement and behavior problems. Washington, DC: National Association for School Psychologists. Hains, A.H., & Baer, D.M. (1989). Interaction effects in multielement designs: Inevitable, desirable, and ignorable. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 5 7 - 6 9 . Johnson, D.W., Johnson, R.T., & Maruyama, G. (1983). Interdependence and interpersonal attraction among heterogeneous and homogeneous individuals: A theoretical formulation and a meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 53, 5 - 5 4 . Kagan, S. (1989-90). The structural approach to cooperative learning. Educational Leadership, 47(4), 1 2 - 1 5 . Siegel, S., & Castellan, N.J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Slavin, R.E. (1983). Student team learning: An overview and practical guide. Washington, DC: National Education Association. Slavin, R.E. (1990). Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Tawney, J.W., & Gast, D.L. (1984). Single-subject research in special education. Columbus, OH: Merrill. Thomas, E., & Beattie, S. (1983). Regions and social needs in communities. Boston: Ginn & Company. Woolfolk, A. (1989). Educational psychology (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Moving? R e m e m b e r to notify y o u r post office o f the n e w address, so that you m a y c o n t i n u e to receive Remedial and Special Education and o t h e r periodicals without interruption. T o help us k e e p our r e c o r d s accurate, s e n d us the address label from your m o s t recent issue plus your n e w address. Allow six w e e k s for an address change. Downloaded from rse.sagepub.com at University of Manitoba Libraries on February 17, 2015