Headache Series Editor: Paolo Martelletti Dimos D. Mitsikostas Fabrizio Benedetti Editors Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches Headache Series Editor Paolo Martelletti Roma, Italy The purpose of this Series, endorsed by the European Headache Federation (EHF), is to describe in detail all aspects of headache disorders that are of importance in primary care and the hospital setting, including pathophysiology, diagnosis, management, comorbidities, and issues in particular patient groups. A key feature of the Series is its multidisciplinary approach, and it will have wide appeal to internists, rheumatologists, neurologists, pain doctors, general practitioners, primary care givers, and pediatricians. Readers will find that the Series assists not only in understanding, recognizing, and treating the primary headache disorders, but also in identifying the potentially dangerous underlying causes of secondary headache disorders and avoiding mismanagement and overuse of medications for acute headache, which are major risk factors for disease aggravation. Each volume is designed to meet the needs of both more experienced professionals and medical students, residents, and trainees. More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/11801 Dimos D. Mitsikostas · Fabrizio Benedetti Editors Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches Editors Dimos D. Mitsikostas Neurology Department, Aeginition Hospital National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Athens Greece Fabrizio Benedetti Neuroscience Department University of Turin Medical School Torino Italy ISSN 2197-652X ISSN 2197-6538 (electronic) Headache ISBN 978-3-030-02975-3 ISBN 978-3-030-02976-0 (eBook) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0 Library of Congress Control Number: 2019930284 © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland Foreword This volume of the EHF Headache Series is dedicated to placebo and nocebo in headaches, with Dimos-Dimitrios Mitsikostas and Fabrizio Benedetti as editors. Their acknowledged expertise in this area of clinical medicine is a guarantee of completeness and deepening of a topic that, in headaches, is rightly considered fundamental. The randomized control trials present today small margins of difference between active substance and the response of healthy controls, and such a small difference, although statistically significant, makes the fundamental role of placebo evident. The thaumaturgical role of the doctor, the protected context of a health environment, and the patient’s expectations have produced brilliant results far beyond the real effectiveness of the prescribed drug. On the other hand, the nocebo effect can balance or limit drug outcomes and also decrease the patient’s therapeutic adherence. Nocebo and placebo effects exert their function by activating a complex interweaving of neural circuits in the central nervous system that modulate the perception of pain. The headache expert needs a reading of this volume to be able to act wisely through the right use of the placebo effect, saving and limiting pharmacological prescriptions where possible, providing for and accompanying the nocebo effect where expected. Paolo Rome, Italy Martelletti v Preface Headaches, migraine, and cluster headache, in particular, are major health problems almost completely underestimated and underappreciated by health-care providers, decision-makers, people, and patients themselves eventually. Recent international epidemiological studies using disability metrics appointed migraine as the second leading cause of disability among all medical conditions after low back pain, but first for people younger than 50. The economic burden of migraine exceeds €18 billion annually in Europe, yet there is no global strategic plan to border the consequences, nor the causes of the conditions. As many other chronic pain disorders, headache affects life quality dramatically, requiring efficient therapeutic procedures. For decades, migraine and cluster headache have being sharing the privilege to be treated with disease-specific acute medications targeting the serotonin 1B and 1D receptor subtypes, but only recently a mechanism-based preventative treatment appeared in the horizon. The anti-CGRP class drugs, both as small molecules and as large monoclonal antibodies, limit migraines and migraine progression into chronic form, as phase 2 and 3 studies have shown. It is a wonderful moment for scientists, for headache specialists, and for patients in particular to live this explosive year that only rarely happens in neurology. However, therapeutics does not end by the right diagnosis and the choice of appropriate medicines. It goes further into the adherence to the treatment, into the parallel management of potential comorbid conditions and finally into a better understanding of the patients’ needs and expectations. Within this context, placebos and nocebos play a fundamental role. Whereas placebos potentially enhance medications’ efficacy, nocebos limit outcomes and decrease adherence. Both are controlled by expectations and conditioning mainly. Because individual perception of pain is also controlled by similar factors, placebos and nocebos have important implications for chronic pain conditions. Headaches and migraine above all carry an essential symbolism, since pain and mind are tightly interconnected. This book is aimed at focusing on both placebos and nocebos and at covering the entire spectrum of headaches, although most data are related to migraine. Top specialists have been invited to present their work in this field, and we are deeply thankful they accepted our invitation and devoted their precious time to vii viii Preface this project. We also thank Madona Samuel and Donatella Rizza from Springer for their professional administrative skills. We hope that the following data will contribute to a better understanding of the mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects in headaches and that all physicians, medical students, neurologists, general practitioners, and headache specialists will improve the clinical management of headaches in everyday clinical practice. Likewise, we hope that patients will get some benefit from this project. Athens, Greece Torino, Italy Dimos D. Mitsikostas Fabrizio Benedetti Contents 1Patient-Centred Care in Headaches �������������������������������������������������������� 1 Christian Lampl and Elisabeth Bräutigam 2Unmet Needs in Headache Management ������������������������������������������������ 13 Andrea Negro and Paolo Martelletti 3Mechanisms of Pain and Headache �������������������������������������������������������� 27 Alexandre F. M. DaSilva and Marcos Fabio DosSantos 4Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo ������������������������������������������������������ 43 Elisa Carlino, Lene Vase, and Alessandro Piedimonte 5The Special Case of High-Altitude Headache ���������������������������������������� 57 Diletta Barbiani, Eleonora Camerone, and Fabrizio Benedetti 6Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache �������������������������������� 65 Jakob Møller Hansen and Messoud Ashina 7Nocebo in Headache Treatment �������������������������������������������������������������� 75 Christina Deligianni and Dimos D. Mitsikostas 8Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents ������������ 85 Vanda Faria and David Borsook 9Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders ���������������������������������� 103 Panagiotis Zis 10Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research ���������������� 113 Luana Colloca and Nathaniel Haycock 11Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice ������������������ 125 Dimos D. Mitsikostas 12Informed Consent and the Ethics of Placebo-Based Interventions in Clinical Practice ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 135 Marco Annoni and Franklin G. Miller ix Chapter 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches Christian Lampl and Elisabeth Bräutigam 1.1 Introduction The World Health Organization (WHO) has acknowledged headache disorders as of global public health importance [38, 39]. However, headache disorders are still under-diagnosed and mostly undertreated [34, 35], not because diagnosis is particularly difficult or because effective treatments do not exist but because of widespread failure of health services to recognize the need for individualized health care for headache sufferers and to take steps to deliver it [35]. These disorders give rise directly, but intermittently, to symptom burden: pain, often accompanied in the case of migraine by nausea, vomiting, and photo- and/or phonophobia. All of these tend to cause debility, prostration and reduced functional ability, a secondary disability burden which is the principal cause of world of years of healthy life lost to disability (YLD) and consequential lost productivity [37]. Those in whom they occur frequently are very likely to worry about when the next headache may happen, and in some this can reach a level of anxiety. More commonly it may provoke avoidance behaviour, particularly among those with migraine who identify triggers and endeavour to eliminate them by lifestyle compromise. Sensible this may be, but too much lifestyle compromise may take the pleasure out of life. The recent Global Burden of Disease Survey 2015 (GBD 2015) [12] found tension-­type headache (TTH) and migraine to be the second and third most prevalent disorders in the world and migraine the seventh highest specific cause of disability [36]. Estimates of disability due to disease are a principal objective of the Global Burden of Disease (GBD) studies, performed since 1990 and described now as “the most comprehensive worldwide observational epidemiological study to date”. Headache disorders account for more disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) C. Lampl (*) · E. Bräutigam Headache Medical Center, Ordensklinikum Linz, Barmherzige Schwestern, Linz, Austria e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_1 1 2 C. Lampl and E. Bräutigam than all other neurological disorders combined (including dementias), despite having no association with mortality [36]. While a wide range of effective pharmacological and non-pharmacological therapies are available for treatment and prevention of headaches, current treatment especially of chronic headache is frequently suboptimal [15–17]. Many patients with debilitating headaches fail to consult a doctor, and those who do may not have their headaches properly identified or treated. 1.2 he Attempt of a Definition of Patient-Centred T Care (PCC) PCC has no globally accepted definition. PCC considers the patient’s standpoint and circumstances during the decision-making process and extends beyond simply setting goals with the patient [27]. This care could also be referred to as a style of doctor-patient encounter characterized by responsiveness to patient needs and preferences using the patient’s informed wishes to guide activity, interaction, and information-­giving and share decision-making [29]. PCC is an approach of viewing health and illness that affects a person’s general well-being and an attempt to empower the patient by expanding his or her role in the patient’s health care. Enhancing the patient’s awareness and providing reassurance, support, comfort, acceptance, legitimacy, and confidence are the basic functions of PCC. A relatively unanimous definition on PCC is “health care that establishes a partnership among practitioners, patients, and their families (when appropriate) to ensure that decisions respect patients’ wants, needs, and preferences”. This definition emphasizes the involvement of patients in the medical decision-making process. However, some definitions seem ambiguous on patient’s preferences, that is, whether patient’s preferences are just one consideration or the ultimate decisive factor in care provision. 1.3 he Importance of Recognizing Patients’ Health T Problems as They See Them as Headache Patients In the past, modern health care has been evolving from the traditional disease-­centred model of care to a more PCC model, which provides a better approach to individualized patient treatment and education. PCC requires adequate recognition of health problems experienced by people suffering from headache. Care is better when it recognizes what headache patients’ problems are, rather than what the diagnosis is [33]. The challenge is to do better at recognizing and documenting their problems. Assessing quality of problem recognition requires documenting the problems and how they change in response to what clinicians do. Headache diagnoses are professional interpretations of observations, of patient descriptions, and—increasingly—of technical investigations and laboratory values. A few primary care researchers in various 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches 3 countries have been trying at least since the late 1980s to understand the relationship between presenting headache symptoms and eventual diagnoses. There is still poor understanding of this relationship, and the more it is neglected, the less attention can be focused on problem reduction over time as a legitimate goal of treatment. Most studies of PCC are carried out in settings involving visits [20]. Prompted by a perceived poor understanding of the term, it has to be asserted that PCC is “determined by the quality of interactions between patients and clinicians” and indicated that they equate patient-centeredness with communication skills, which “are a fundamental component of the approach to care that is characterized by continuous healing relationships, shared understanding, emotional support, trust, patient enablement and activation, and informed choices” [18]. So the concept of PCC may be one fundamental way of improving the quality of care in headache patients. 1.4 General Aspects of PCC There are many different aspects of PCC, including: –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– –– Respecting people’s values and putting people at the centre of care Taking into account people’s preferences and expressed needs Coordinating and integrating care Working together to make sure there is good communication, information, and education Making sure people are physically comfortable and safe Emotional support Involving family and friends Making sure there is continuity between and within services Making sure people have access to appropriate care when they need it PCC is a way of thinking and doing things that sees the people using health and social services as equal partners in planning, developing, and monitoring care to make sure it meets their needs. This means putting people and their families at the centre of decisions and seeing them as experts, working alongside professionals to get the best outcome. PCC is not just about giving people whatever they want or providing information. It is about considering people’s desires, values, family situations, social circumstances, and lifestyles; seeing the person as an individual, with a high burden of the disease(s); and working together to develop appropriate solutions. 1.5 PCC in Headaches There can be no doubt that migraine and medication overuse headache are a major contributor to public ill health in all countries, climes, and cultures. There is evidence that in a considerable proportion of people with migraine and in a small proportion of 4 C. Lampl and E. Bräutigam people with TTH, ictal and interictal burden are real and measurable [36]. The significance of interictal burden is that, although it may be at relatively low level, it is present for longer periods of time than ictal burden [17]. Whereas the ictal burden of episodic headache is typically present during only 1 or 2 days in every month, interictal burden can impose itself on all of the other days. This means two things. First, interictal burden ought not to be ignored: the burden of headache is very poorly described if it does not take interictal burden into account. Second, if interictal burden is overestimated and then multiplied by time, quantification of overall burden is likely to be greatly distorted. This should also be taken into account when performing PCC. PCC seems to be a promising avenue for headache management. In order to improve understanding of how caregivers could use PCC more effectively in headache patients, headache researchers need rigorous methods to capture the different components of this approach. 1.6 Nocebo in PCC For drug-nonspecific effects causing unwanted side effects, the corresponding term of nocebo has been established [1]. The nocebo effect has impact on therapeutic response, adherence, and quality of life. The nocebo response is influenced by the content and the way information is presented to patients in clinical practice. Specifically, research on the nocebo effect indicates that information disclosure about potential side effects can itself contribute to producing adverse effects [7]. On the other hand, informing a patient that a prescribed drug may cause side effects may itself produce the same side effects independent of the pharmacological properties of the drug. But it is not only the drug; it is also the doctor: just as the interpersonal and environmental dimensions of the clinician have a potentially powerful therapeutic benefit [5, 19], negative aspects of the clinician can have negative, nocebo effects. In daily clinical practice, nocebo effects can be a result of the interactions between clinicians and patients. Clinicians have an obligation to bring adequate, truthful information to their patients so that they can make individualized decisions about their medical care. One of the key purposes of informed consent is to communicate to patients the benefits and risks of recommended treatment interventions, so that they can decide for themselves whether undergoing these treatments is a reasonable option for them [7]. This information can be conveyed “negatively” (by focusing on the minority of patients who experience a particular side effect) or “positively” (by focusing on the majority of patients who do not experience the side effect). These different ways of framing side effect information may have differential effects on patients with respect to forming nocebo responses [22]. 1.7 Placebo in PCC It is evident that placebo effects play a crucial role in the treatment process, and its effects remain a key contributor to patient well-being and treatment outcome [6, 9, 11]. Placebo effects can be defined as the component of a medical treatment effect 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches 5 that is directly attributable to patients’ expectations of and prior learning experience with a therapeutic intervention [10]. Experimental studies and randomized placebo-­ controlled clinical trials (RCTs) are the two sources our knowledge of placebo effects primarily comes from. Although the goals and approaches are distinct, a shared characteristic is that treatment randomization and distribution occur without any participant/patient involvement (so they have a passive role). However, as part of a larger movement towards PCC, the absence of participant/patient involvement in treatment decisions in placebo studies and RCTs stands in contrast to many practices and trends in modern health care. And this leads us to the right way: research now indicates that the magnitude of placebo effects differs depending upon the amount of participant involvement via decisional control. In PCC the relationship between the physician and the patient should be seen (and make used) as a social mechanism for beneficial impact on the patient. Therefore it is more than important to realize why this social interplay is necessary to stimulate the endogenous mechanisms that handle expectation and placebo outcomes. According to Humphrey [14], the ability to stimulate expectation in addition to placebo mechanisms following the doctor-patient encounter is an emergent issue and essential feature of the “natural health-care service”. The conceptualization of an endogenous health-care system is extremely useful to know why the doctor-patient encounter is necessary in order to trigger expectation in addition to placebo mechanism in the patient’s brain. From the PCC perspective, it is obvious that the physician belongs to the system and has a pivotal role in triggering all mechanisms that take place in the patient’s brain [21]. Therefore it is of great importance to involve the patient via choice making. Patient’s participation in treatment decisions or planning may strengthen placebo effects and should enhance treatment effectiveness. This provides a clear avenue for harnessing placebo effects in clinical practice. As patients’ role in health care has increased, understanding the connections between patient involvement and placebo effects will be vital for PCC [13]. 1.8 Successful Headache Management It is essential for an appropriate headache management to start with a patient and provider collaborative partnership, with a thoughtful understanding of the individual’s needs, preferences, and values. In addition, it takes open communication that encourages information sharing, choices, and mutual respect that leads to patient empowerment, self-responsibility, and at least self-management of their headache(s). This patient-centred approach must to be integrated into an evidencebased system that delivers sound scientific basis for headache treatment. Combining PCC and evidence-based practice provides the pinnacle of quality and value in headache care [28]. First step of PCC in headache is patient elucidation and education; here within individualized patient education is the method of first choice. This includes accurate headache diagnosis—nearly half of migraineurs who seek medical care in a given year are unaware of their migraine diagnosis [2]; thus, it is essential to provide and 6 C. Lampl and E. Bräutigam discuss headache diagnosis at each appointment—discussion about appropriate treatment options, and information about trigger (environmental, medication, emotional, physical, or dietary influences that can potentially cause a migraine attack) and lifestyle factors (the broad subject of lifestyle management included diet, exercise, sleep hygiene, and stress) which can directly affect migraine frequency. Knowing person’s trigger and lifestyle factors is crucial to prevent migraine attacks. The main goals are to increase patient’s knowledge and understanding about the disease and show skills to independently manage their headache. To achieve these it is of importance to establish headache diary information and initial assessment. For example, tracking the migraine trigger factors through a diary is important in the evaluation of migraine headaches in order to get a better understanding of the relationship of the triggers to migraine occurrences. A first study comparing the assessment of lifestyle and trigger factors in retrospective questionnaires to daily diaries showed diverging results [40]. Questionnaires and diaries correlated for lifestyle factors in 91% but for trigger factors in only 33%. The poor correlation between questionnaire and diary regarding triggers remained unchanged with a more sophisticated approach, i.e. the calculation of odd ratios for each factor. Accordingly, assessing lifestyle factors in migraine patients by means of a questionnaire is highly reliable, but there is an inconsistency between questionnaires and diaries regarding trigger factors. The headache diary is also used to keep track of medication changes, keeping count of abortive medications used (if needed), as well as the frequency and intensity of headache(s) which helps to provide accurate information to bring to clinic appointments. However, the first European study to evaluate headache care quality indicators across a culturally diverse multinational range of settings showed lack of systematic use of diagnostic diaries and disability and quality-of-life assessment instruments and restricted opportunities for follow-up visits [31]. The same may apply to quality-of-life assessment, a problem in almost all centres, although this is more relevant to outcome assessment during follow-up than to the formulation of treatment plans at time of presentation. The mind-set of most of the centres appeared to focus on treatment of patients’ symptoms. Most patients suffering from headache who receive timely and good-quality health care can expect this care to be effective, but good quality in health care has not been the automatic result of the marked changes in scope, character, and content of headache practice and care that have occurred during the last years. Quality of care has not been much subject to social awareness or interest: a collaborative EHF/ LTB project of which the study [31] was part of it is a step towards bringing headache service quality centre-stage. This takes importance from the fact that there has been no similar initiative preceding it—individualized PPC could be one. Besides individualized PCC education sessions may also be a possible way of headache patient education (particularly in case of lack of resources to provide patient education). Rothrock et al. [30] employed a group intervention at a headache specialty clinic using lay (non-professional) instructors as educators citing a very large migraine patient population and patient education. They also provided information regarding when to treat migraine abortively with the emphasis on treating early in the course of migraine as leaving migraine progress in severity without 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches 7 proper treatment will lead to poor outcomes. Cady et al. [4] research took place at a headache specialty clinic using a 12-min video of migraine pathophysiology (with and without a nurse present), identifying which phases of migraine are best to initiate migraine abortive treatment. The study showed that having the presence of a nurse during the education video was more effective than no support. Several of the studies placed a major focus on stress management by providing a behavioural skills component to the patient education intervention (e.g. relaxation techniques, behavioural therapies, coping techniques, biofeedback audio tapes). With interest one study [5] took the stress management emphasis, offering a 4-h-long session on Acceptance and Commitment Training (ACT) which is a behavioural therapy that incorporates acceptance and mindfulness strategies for behavioural changes used in depression and anxiety which is a known comorbidity with migraine. Many of the studies incorporated headache medication and non-pharmacological treatments into the intervention in various ways, but none of the information was well defined [3, 4, 8, 19, 30, 32]. Interestingly only one study provided information regarding medication overuse headaches (MOH) and how to prevent them by using proper medication management [8]. But, this is again a very important issue to discuss with the patient. It is well known that chronic migraine represents migraine natural evolution from its episodic form. It is realized through a chronification phase that may require months or years and varies from patient to patient. The transition to more frequent attacks pattern is influenced by lifestyle, life events, comorbid conditions, and personal genetic terrain, and it often leads to acute drugs overuse. MOH may complicate every type of headache, and all the drugs employed for headache treatment can cause MOH [23]. MOH is more a complication of chronic migraine than a single entity. MOH can be minimized by patient education at the very first on and by treating them with prophylactics in a proper way. PCC is not only a pretreatment necessity; moreover it is an “in-between companionship and monitoring”. Since headaches, especially migraine, chronic migraine, MOH, and trigeminal-autonomic cephalalgias (TACs), are influenced with a high personal and interpersonal disability, disability monitoring and outcome assessments are essential to measure patient education outcomes. The Migraine Disability Assessment (MIDAS) measurement questionnaire, which is well known for its reliability and validity in evaluating migraine disability, evaluates migraine disability within a 3-month period of time. Others are the Headache Impact Test (HIT-6) or the Headache Disability Inventory (without known reliability or validity). Measuring disability using a valid and reliable migraine disability tool is needed to evaluate patient education outcome measures to determine whether the patient education activities are successful and migraine management is suitable for all individuals. For the MIDAS all studies using this method showed statistically significant positive findings [19, 24, 30]. There are numerous headache treatment guidelines (nearly in each country in Europe) providing comprehensive migraine management recommendations. However, only a small number endorse initiation of patient education before or shortly after the diagnosis is made. There is a strong recommendation for keeping headache diaries to clarify frequency and severity, headache triggers, and treatment responses. Other patient education recommendations should include understanding 8 C. Lampl and E. Bräutigam headache(s), genetic predisposition, food and environmental triggers, and lifestyle changes related to diet, sleep, stress reduction, and regular aerobic exercise. The importance of limiting abortive migraine medication and to favour prophylactic treatment must be stressed to avoid MOH. There is limited research in the area of PCC in headache and the intention to provide evidence of positive trials related to headache management that could be used in clinical practice. This article was written from a clinician (and headache sufferer) perspective and recommended a multidisciplinary approach to patients suffering from headache(s), obtaining input from the patient, the family, and the provider. All primary headaches are incurable diseases with complex management issues necessitating a patient-centred plan of care that can be used as a comprehensive reference tool to assist in controlling headache(s) and improving quality of life. It is also important to stress the importance of addressing headache comorbidities like depression and anxiety, if needed, by prescribing antidepressant medication and relaxation techniques such as biofeedback. Reviewing the current literature on headache and the knowledge gap pertaining to those who may not be receiving the knowledge and skills necessary to self-­ manage their headache, particularly individuals with low reading levels, language barriers, or cultural differences could heighten health-care disparities and patient dissatisfaction. The Matchar et al. study [19] had the strongest research evidence for a PCC model, which included patient stakeholder involvement with a patient education program offering individualized and (if not otherwise possible) group patient education programs with personalized plans of care. These should include the following: 1. Patient education should take place at regularly scheduled office visits by the headache patient’s neurology provider. 2. Individualized headache plan of care that would outline the patient’s treatment should be provided for the patient to take home. 3. Patient education materials that would be easy to understand for all individuals should be provided. 4. Patient education information that includes recognizing different headache symptoms, how to prevent headache by avoiding triggers, use of a headache diary, how to manage headache at home, and understanding medication and non-­ medication treatments should be provided. 5. The principles of health equity should be embedded in all headache patient education methods to ensure proper education to all diverse populations. 1.9 Conclusion Reviews of research about this topic found that offering PCC in headache sufferers usually improves outcomes [25]. Good-quality headache care achieves accurate diagnosis and individualized management, has appropriate referral pathways, 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches 9 educates patients about their headache(s) and their management, is convenient and comfortable, satisfies patients, is efficient and equitable, assesses outcomes, and is safe [26]. Research has found that PCC in headache patients can have a big impact on the quality of care. It can improve the experience people have of care and help them feel more satisfied. It can encourage people with headache to lead a more healthy lifestyle, such as exercising or eating healthily; it should encourage people to be more involved in decisions about their care, so they get services and support that are appropriate for their needs. This may in turn reduce the overall cost of care. But there is not as much evidence about this improves how confident and satisfied professionals themselves feel about the care provided. While the evidence is mounting that PCC can make a difference, there are not that many studies about outcomes yet and some research has mixed. This makes it even more important to think about how to measure and put PCC into headache practice, so that health services can better understand the benefits of this. References 1. Barsky AJ, Saintfort R, Rogers MP, Borus JF. Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon. JAMA. 2002;28:622–7. 2. Bigal M, Krymchantowski AV, Lipton RB. Barriers to satisfactory migraine outcomes. What we have learned, where do we stand? Headache. 2009;49:1028–41. 3. Bromberg J, Wood ME, Black RA, Surette DA, Zacharoff KL, Chiauzzi EJ. A randomized trial of a web-based intervention to improve migraine self management and coping. Headache. 2012;52:244–61. 4. Cady J, Farmer K, Beach ME, Tarrasch J. Nurse-based education: an office based comparative model for education of migraine patients. Headache. 2007;48:564–9. 5. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:679–84. 6. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebos and painkillers: is mind as real as matter? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:545–52. 7. Colloca L, Miller FG. The nocebo effect and its relevance for clinical practice. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:598–603. 8. Dindo L, Recober A, Marchman JN, Turvey C, O’Hara MW. One-day behavioural treatment for patients with comorbid depression and migraine: a pilot study. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50:537–43. 9. Enck P, Bingel U, Schedlowski M, Rief W. The placebo response in medicine: minimize, maximize, or personalize? Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2013;12:191–204. 10. Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, Benedetti F. Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet. 2010;375(9715):686–95. 11. Flaten MA, Simonsen T, Olsen H. Drug-related information generates placebo and nocebo responses that modify the drug response. Psychosom Med. 1999;61:250–5. 12. GBD 2015 Neurological Disorders Collaborator Group. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders during 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2015. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:877–97. 13. Geers AL, Rose JP, Brown JA. Aligning research and practice: implications of patient-centered care for placebo effects. Patient. 2014;7:1–3. 14. Humphrey N. The mind made flesh. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. 10 C. Lampl and E. Bräutigam 15. Hu HX, Solomon GD, Conboy K, Deml L, Markson LE. Impact of a migraine disease management program. Dis Manag Health Out. 2004;12:273–80. 16. Katsarava Z, Mania M, Lampl C, Herberhold J, Steiner TJ. Poor medical care for people with migraine in Europe—evidence from the Eurolight study. J Headache Pain. 2018;19(1):10. 17. Lampl C, Thomas H, Stovner LJ, Tassorelli C, Katsarava Z, Laínez JM, Lantéri-Minet M, Rastenyte D, Ruiz de la Torre E, Andrée C, Steiner TJ. Interictal burden attributable to episodic headache: findings from the Eurolight project. J Headache Pain. 2016;17:9. 18. Levinson W, Lesser CS, Epstein RM. Developing physician communication skills for patient-­ centred care. Health Aff (Millwood). 2010;29:1310–8. 19. Matchar DB, Harpole L, Samsa GP, Jurgelski A, Lipton RB, Silberstein SD, Blumenfeld A. The headache management trial: a randomized study of coordinated care. Headache. 2008;48:1294–310. 20. Mead N, Bower P, Hann M. The impact of general practitioners’ patient-centredness on patients’ post-consultation satisfaction and enablement. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55:283–99. 21. Miller FG, Kaptchuk TJ. The power of context: reconceptualizing the placebo effect. J R Soc Med. 2008;101:222–5. 22. Miller FG, Colloca L. The placebo phenomenon and medical ethics: rethinking the relationship between informed consent and risk-benefit assessment. Theor Med Bioeth. 2011;32:229–43. 23. Negro A, Martelletti P. Chronic migraine plus medication overuse headache: two entities or not? J Headache Pain. 2011;12:593–601. 24. Nicholson R, Nash J, Andrasik F. A self-administered behavioral intervention using tailored messages for migraine. Headache. 2005;45:1124–39. 25. Olsson LE, Jakobsson Ung E, Swedberg K, Ekman I. Efficacy of person-centred care as an intervention in controlled trials—a systematic review. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22:456–65. 26. Peters M, Perera S, Loder E, Jenkinson C, Gil Gouveia R, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Steiner TJ. Quality in the provision of headache care. Systematic review of the literature and commentary. J Headache Pain. 2012;13:437–47. 27. Ponte PR, Conlin G, Conway JB, Grant S, Medeiros C, Nies J, Shulman L, Branowicki P, Conley K. Making patient-centered care come alive: achieving full integration of the patient’s perspective. J Nurs Adm. 2003;33:82–90. 28. Rechtzigel AD. Patient-centred migraine management. http://sophia.stkate.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1051&context=dnp_projects. 29. Rogers A, Kennedy A, Nelson E, Robinson A. Uncovering the limits of patient-centeredness: implementing a self-management trial for chronic illness. Qual Health Res. 2005;15:224–39. 30. Rothrock JF, Parada VA, Sims C, Key K, Walters NS, Zweifler RM. The impact of intensive patient education on clinical outcome in a clinic-based migraine population. Headache. 2006;46:726–31. 31. Schramm S, Uluduz D, Gouveia RG, Jensen R, Siva A, Uygunoglu U, Gvantsa G, Mania M, Braschinsky M, Filatova E, Latysheva N, Osipova V, Skorobogatykh K, Azimova J, Straube A, Eren OE, Martelletti P, De Angelis V, Negro A, Linde M, Hagen K, Radojicic A, Zidverc-­ Trajkovic J, Podgorac A, Paemeleire K, De Pue A, Lampl C, Steiner TJ, Katsarava Z. Headache service quality: evaluation of quality indicators in 14 specialist-care centres. J Headache Pain. 2016;17:111. 32. Smith TR, Nicholson RA, Banks JW. A primary care migraine education program has benefit on headache impact and quality of life: results from the mercy migraine management program. Headache. 2010;50:1–19. 33. Starfield B. Primary care and equity in health: the importance to effectiveness and equity of responsiveness to peoples’ needs. Humanity Soc. 2009;33:56–73. 34. Steiner TJ. Lifting the burden: the global campaign to reduce the burden of headache worldwide. J Headache Pain. 2005;14:373–7. 35. Steiner TJ, Birbeck GL, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Martelletti P, Stovner LJ. Lifting the burden: the first 7 years. J Headache Pain. 2010;15:451–5. 1 Patient-Centred Care in Headaches 11 36. Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Vos T. GBD 2015: migraine is the third cause of disability in under 50 years. J Headache Pain. 2016;17:104. 37. Stovner LJ, Hagen K, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Lipton R, Scher AI, et al. The global burden of headache: a documentation of headache prevalence and disability worldwide. Cephalalgia. 2007;14:193–210. 38. World Health Organization. The world health report 2001. Geneva: WHO; 2001. p. 19–45. 39. World Health Organization and Lifting The Burden. Atlas of headache disorders and resources in the world 2011. Geneva: WHO; 2011. 40. Zebenholzer K, Frantal S, Pablik E, Lieba-Samal D, Salhofer-Polanyi S, Wöber-Bingöl C, Wöber C. Reliability of assessing lifestyle and trigger factors in patients with migraine—findings from the PAMINA study. Eur J Neurol. 2016;23:120–6. Chapter 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management Andrea Negro and Paolo Martelletti 2.1 Acute Treatment in Migraine Acute treatments are aimed to abolish ongoing attacks, reduce migraine-related symptoms, and improve functional disability. Those treatments include migrainespecific medications (i.e., triptans, ergotamine, and dihydroergotamine) and nonspecific agents (i.e., nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs [NSAIDs], simple and combination analgesics, opioids, barbiturates, antihistamines, antinauseants, and muscle relaxants). The choice of an acute therapy has important limitations in patients at risk or after transient coronary heart disease, ischemic attack (TIA), ischemic stroke, or cerebral bleeds. Triptans and ergots are contraindicated to treat migraine attacks in patients with a history of stroke, coronary heart disease, or not-controlled hypertension, while aspirin and NSAIDs should not be used in patients with a history of cerebral hemorrhage. 2.2 Preventive Treatment in Migraine A preventive therapy is required when the frequency of the attacks is ≥4 per month. Prophylactic migraine treatments are given on a daily basis to prevent the occurrence of migraine attacks, to reduce headache frequency and severity, and to decrease associated disability and the need for acute medications which may be contributing to concurrent medication overuse headache (MOH). A. Negro (*) · P. Martelletti Department of Clinical and Molecular Medicine, Regional Referral Headache Centre, Sapienza University of Rome, Sant’Andrea Hospital, Rome, Italy e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_2 13 14 A. Negro and P. Martelletti Several pharmacological classes can be used for migraine prevention: antihypertensive agents (e.g., β-blockers, calcium channel blockers, angiotensin-converting enzyme [ACE] inhibitors, aldosterone receptor blockers), antiepileptic drugs (e.g., topiramate, divalproex sodium), tricyclic antidepressants (e.g., amitriptyline, nortriptyline), selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) [65]. OnabotulinumtoxinA (Botox®) is indicated for the prevention of chronic migraine (CM) and is the only FDA-approved treatment for this indication. As for acute treatment, the choice of the best preventive therapy requires clinical considerations. Migraine is frequently associated with several comorbidities that it is important to consider in treatment decision-making. A therapy, particularly those used for prevention, could be contraindicated in presence of a comorbid disease where another therapy could be effective to treat both migraine and the other condition. Moreover, patients with migraine, in particular migraine with aura, have a higher risk of ischemic [69] and hemorrhagic stroke [6] as well as cerebral hemorrhage [45]. Migraineurs needing prevention and having cardiovascular comorbidities should be treated with beta-blockers, candesartan, or lisinopril, while NSAIDs and SSRIs are contraindicated as prevention in patients with a history of cerebral bleeds. 2.3 Physician-Patient Communication: Treatment Decision-Making The quality of physician-patient interaction can improve patient outcomes. Several studies proved that attempts to positively influence patients about the effects of therapy had a significant impact on health outcomes [26, 32, 36, 76, 77]. There are three models for medical treatment decision-making. The “paternalistic model,” in which decisions are taken by the physician without considering patient’s preferences, has been replaced by more patient-centered approaches [15]. In the “informed model,” the patient makes a treatment decision after being informed about treatment options, risks, and benefits. In the “shared model,” after discussing treatment, both physician and patient actively participate in making a shared medical decision. In case of triptan prescription, nearly the totality of patients (92%) prefers the decision to be joint with the provider [56]. Physician-patient communication and patients’ involvement and agreement on care and treatment plan are associated with improved patient adherence and compliance with prescribed medication, improved satisfaction, resolution of emotional and physical symptoms, and fewer repeated consultations, referrals, and investigations [35, 44, 57, 74]. However, in actual practice, involvement in decision-making is poor, with patients reporting that for triptan prescription, the physician was the sole decision-maker 55.1% of the time [56]. 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 2.4 15 Patients’ Expectations: What Migraineurs Want Patients’ satisfaction is determined by both therapeutic reality and expectations. The most frequent expectations about the attributes of migraine medications reported by patients include effective and long-lasting pain relief, low rate of migraine recurrence, few side effects, easy route of administration, low number of doses needed to achieve pain relief, effective relief of associated symptoms, and fast return to normal activities [24]. Understanding patients’ preferences regarding education and their role in decision-making when treatments are prescribed can enhance satisfaction and adherence with treatment plans. The majority of patients want to participate in decision-making when a triptan is prescribed and prefer to receive education about the triptan from the prescribing provider [56]. The most desired topics for education included if/when a triptan should be taken and how many times for each migraine, how many doses can be taken each week/month, what to do in case of inefficacy, co-administration with other acute medications, and possible side effects [56]. Patients’ expectations are not limited to the prescribed treatment but also to the quality of the headache services. Quality indicators for headache centers include the availability of referral pathway from primary to specialist care and urgent referral pathway when necessary, a service environment that is clean and comfortable, short waiting times in the clinic, and sufficient time allocated to each visit [68]. 2.5 Barriers in Episodic Migraine Management An adequate migraine management requires three crucial steps: (1) medical consultation, (2) correct diagnosis, and (3) acute pharmacologic therapy. Failure at any level makes good clinical outcomes unlikely. The American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) study sample was used to identify barriers to care in any of the three steps in a population of individuals with episodic migraine (EM) with unmet treatment needs, defined by a Migraine Disability Assessment Scale (MIDAS) score of mild-to-severe grade [52]. Only one quarter of individuals successfully completed all three steps. The strongest predictors of medical consultation were having health insurance (OR 1.73), elevated headache-related disability (OR 1.06 for a 10-point change in MIDAS score), and high composite migraine symptom severity score (OR 1.19). Among consulters, 86.7% reported receiving a medical diagnosis of migraine, which was more likely in women than in men (OR 4.25) suggesting that gender bias in diagnosis may be an important barrier for men. Diagnosis was more likely in those with higher annual household incomes (OR 1.94) and became increasingly likely with increasing average headache pain severity (OR 1.44). Among the diagnosed patients, 66.7% used acute migraine treatments. The strongest predictors of treatments were annual 16 A. Negro and P. Martelletti household income (OR 1.44) followed by a high headache-related disability (OR 1.16 for a 10-point change in MIDAS score). 2.6 Barriers in Chronic Migraine Management Regarding CM patients with unmet needs, the issue of the barriers to medical care was investigated by the Chronic Migraine Epidemiology and Outcomes (CaMEO) study [27]. When comparing the results with those in EM patients from the AMPP study, what emerges for CM patients is an even larger unmet need for improving care. Less than 5% of persons with CM traversed three barriers to receiving care for headache (vs. 26% in EM). Consultation rates were lower for people with CM than EM (41% vs. 46%), and among consulters the diagnostic rates were also lower for those with CM than EM (25% vs. 87%). Finally, among those who were diagnosed, treatment rates were also lower for those with CM than EM (44% vs. 67%). The strongest predictors of medical consultation among CM patients were age, having health insurance, and greater levels of headache-related disability and symptom severity. An appropriate diagnosis was more likely in those consulting a specialist, in women, and with greater migraine severity. 2.7 reatment Optimization and Progression from EM T to CM The migraine Treatment Optimization Questionnaire in 6 items (mTOQ-6) is a helpful tool to assess response to acute treatment in persons with migraine exploring six domains: (1) Quick Return to Function, (2) 2-h Pain-Free, (3) Sustained 24-h Pain Relief, (4) Tolerability, (5) Comfortable to Make Plans, and (6) Perceived Control. The mTOQ-6 was used to measure acute treatment optimization among EM and CM patients that responded to the 2006 AMPP study survey [72]. Both the EM and CM groups exhibited low rates of treatment optimization, though treatment optimization was worse for CM across all domains. Poor treatment optimization was associated with allodynia, depression, use of NSAIDs or ergotamines, and the absence of a preventive therapy. Patients with EM and poor acute treatment optimization of 1 year have an increased risk of progression to CM the following year [54]. In the AMPP study, the rate of evolution into CM was 1.9% for subjects with maximally optimized treatment, 2.7% for those moderately optimized, 4.4% for those poorly optimized, and 6.8% for those very poorly optimized. Other risk factors for chronicization include the number of migraine attacks, sleep disorders (such as snoring and sleep apnea), obesity, stress, depression, and caffeine use/misuse (dietary and drug-containing caffeine) [64]. 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 17 CM is often complicated by MOH. Two principal factors lead to overuse: (1) the recommendation of an early treatment, increasing the risk to take more of the drug than is necessary; and (2) the acute treatment is partially or not effective. All the acute treatments, when overused, can cause MOH, which in turn decrease responsiveness to acute or prophylactic drugs [66]. Due to an insufficient response to classic therapies, a significant percentage of patients shift in refractoriness, developing what is defined as refractory CM [62]. 2.8 Acute Migraine Treatment: Triptans Triptans are considered the standard of acute therapy for migraine attacks. Nevertheless, the minority of potentially eligible persons use them. The AMPP study showed that in 2005 less than one in five persons with migraine in the United States used triptans over the course of a year [17]. Groups less likely to use triptans included males, African Americans, older adults, and the uninsured. Increased triptan use was associated with several markers of severe headache, including high headache frequency, disability, and allodynia. New triptan use occurred in only 4.9% of the sample surveyed. Starting triptan was lower in patients aged 60 years or older vs. those 18–29 years of age. In adjusted model analysis, age, disability, preventive medication use, income, and insurance were associated with new triptan use [7]. AMPP survey showed that 92% of EM subjects used acute medications, and among them the 52% used multiple classes [51]. Triptans were used by 18.3% of respondents, but only 21.7% of them used triptans as monotherapy, while 38.7% used one additional class of medication, and 39.6% used two or more additional classes [10]. The 57% of triptan users continued their regimen over the course of 1 year, while 14.4% added additional types of medication, and 28.6% discontinued at least one type of medication. Higher education was protective against medication escalation, while depression was found to be a predictor of increasing medication use. Together with older age and allodynia, depression was also a predictor of decreasing medication use. 2.9 Triptan Discontinuation Data from the 2008 and 2009 AMPP surveys were used to assess reasons for discontinuation of two commonly used classes of medication, triptans and opioids, between EM patients [40]. Opioid use was associated with an increased risk of medication discontinuation compared to triptans (59.0% vs. 34.6%). Opioid discontinuers were 52% more likely to discontinue because of pain recurrence than those discontinuing triptans, 53% more likely to discontinue because of concern about interactions with other medications, and 58% more likely due to concern about effects to the stomach. 18 A. Negro and P. Martelletti Factors associated with triptan discontinuation among migraine patients were further investigated by a multicenter cross-sectional survey performed at US tertiary care headache clinics that enrolled EM and CM patients who were current or past triptan users [79]. Discontinuation was most correlated with lack of efficacy (OR 17), greater migraine-related disability (OR 2.6), depression (OR 2.5), and the use of opioids for migraine attacks (OR 2.2). Compared with patients who had discontinued triptans, current users were more likely having a triptan prescribed by a specialist and using other abortive medication with the triptan. Moreover, triptan users felt they had more control over their migraine attack and confidence in their prescribing provider and were educated about their triptan. A recent systematic review showed that 25–60% of patients never refill their index triptan and an annual rate of discontinuation among a general population of triptan users between 30 and 60% [58]. 2.10 2.10.1 How to Improve the Efficacy of Triptans Response Predictors Several studies have examined predictors and rates of successful treatment outcomes for single attacks in clinical trials. Data from 2006 AMPP survey was used to identify predictors of acute treatment success or failure at 2 h and 24 h post dose over multiple migraine attacks [55]. Fifty-six percent of respondents reported inadequate 2-h pain-free (2hPF) response, 53.7% reported inadequate pain relief at 24 h (24hPR), and 25.7% reported inadequate 24-h sustained pain freedom (24hSPR) or recurrence. Predictors for each of the three outcomes included higher migraine frequency and symptoms severity, allodynia, and depression. Other predictors of inadequate 2hPF response included male sex, higher body mass index (BMI), and not using preventive migraine medications, while medication overuse was a predictor of both inadequate 24hPR and 24hSPR. These data are supported by the results of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) that showed 2hPF ranging from 12% (frovatriptan 2.5 mg) to 40% (rizatriptan 10 mg) and 24hSPR ranging from 16% (naratriptan 2.5 mg) to 26% (almotriptan 12.5 mg) [75]. 2.10.2 Choice of the First-Line Therapy Time to treatment and type of first-line therapy play an important role in improving migraine outcomes [46]. On average, migraines treated with a prescription or an over-the-counting (OTC) medication within 1 h of onset are of 3.2 h shorter in duration compared with those treated more than an hour after onset [46]. Migraines treated with triptan as first-line therapy showed better outcomes over those treated with an OTC medication first, as evidenced by a significantly lower proportion requiring a rescue medication, a shorter migraine duration, and a higher proportion of alleviated symptoms within 4 h [46]. 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 2.10.3 19 Time to Treatment Evidence from clinical studies suggest that early treatment and treating while migraine pain is still mild result in improved clinical outcomes [12, 13, 31, 41, 67], but real-world studies have shown that migraineurs tend to delay treatment until pain is severe [29, 31, 41]. Personal beliefs have a strong influence on how migraineurs manage their migraines. The main reasons for waiting before using a triptan are concerns to run out of triptans, concerns about taking medications, concerns about side effects, and fear of developing tolerance to triptans [47]. An observational study conducted in the United States showed that 58.8% of migraineurs delayed triptan treatment, and for the 55% of them, the reason was to be certain that the headache was a migraine [47]. The same study found that the decision to use a triptan immediately was influenced by some migraine-related factors as moderate or severe pain, throbbing or stabbing type of pain, and certain symptoms such as nausea, vomiting, and light and sound sensitivity [47]. However, only the 61% of patients took triptan first, while the 49% took an OTC or non-triptan as first medication [47]. 2.10.4 Switching Acute Treatment Several studies reported that 5–9% of patients initiating a new triptan switch to a different triptan before refilling their original medication [58]. The principal reasons that push patients to switch from a triptan to another triptan include efficacy, consistency, migraine recurrence, curiosity, and formulation [73]. The effect of changes in treatment from one triptan to another or from a triptan to another medication class was investigated using AMPP study data to assess change in headache-related disability [71]. Switching between triptans or from a triptan to another class of medication was not associated with improvements in headache-related disability, but switching from a triptan to an NSAID was associated with significant increases in headache-related disability among patients with high-frequency episodic migraine/chronic migraine (HFEM/CM). 2.10.5 dding Additional Acute Treatments to Current A Triptan Therapy An alternative strategy to increase triptan efficacy could be adding additional acute treatments to current triptan therapy. This option was investigated using data from the AMPP study and assessing change in MIDAS score from the first to the second year of a couplet [11]. Subjects were divided based on headache days per month: low-frequency episodic migraine (LFEM, 0–4), moderate-frequency episodic migraine (MFEM, 5–9), and HFEM/CM (≥10 headache days per month). Adding acute therapies to a current triptan regimen was generally not associated with 20 A. Negro and P. Martelletti reductions in headache-related disability and in some cases was associated with worse outcomes. Adding opioids and barbiturates was not beneficial overall. Adding a second triptan was not beneficial in general, and for patients with HFEM, it was associated with worsening of headache-related disability. Also adding NSAIDs was associated with greater headache-related disability among HFEM/CM patients but was beneficial for the MFEM group. The last finding is supported by prior observations showing that NSAIDs may protect against the progression from EM to CM for MFEM cases [53]. The finding that for HFEM/CM subjects adding a triptan or NSAIDs is associated with an increase in headache-related disability fits with prior reports that frequent triptan and NSAID use in HFEM is associated with the progression to CM [4]. 2.11 Unmet Needs in EM Patients The AMPP study also provided important insights about the relative frequency among persons with EM of five types of unmet treatment needs: (1) dissatisfaction with current acute treatment, (2) moderate or severe headache-related disability (based on MIDAS score), (3) excessive use of or dependence from opioids or barbiturates, (4) recurrent use of the emergency department for headache, and (5) history of cardiovascular events indicating a contraindication to triptan use [4]. Of 5591 subjects with EM, 40.7% had one or more unmet needs. The three most common unmet needs were moderate or severe headache-related disability (47.0%), dissatisfaction with current acute treatment (37.4%), and excessive opioid and/or barbiturate use or probable dependence (32.0%). The odds of having unmet treatment needs were higher in those with more headache days, anxiety, or depression. 2.12 Unmet Needs in CM Patients Chronic migraine interferes with daily activities and causes reduction in their quality of life more than EM does and represents the most important challenge for tertiary-level headache centers [64]. Preventive treatment is the key for a correct CM management. Any of the oral preventive therapies indicated for EM may be prescribed to CM patients, but only the use of onabotulinumtoxinA and topiramate is supported by RCTs [48]. For other medications (e.g., beta-blockers, calcium channel blockers, SSRIs, SNRIs, sodium valproate, gabapentin, amitriptyline, tizanidine, zonisamide) proofs of efficacy come from single randomized controlled trials or open-label studies and are often used on an empirical basis for CM prevention [16]. Migraine, particularly when chronic, is often associated with comorbidities as cardiovascular and psychiatric disorders, fibromyalgia, myofascial pain syndromes, and various forms of visceral pain [64]. The presence of comorbidities should drive the choice of a preventive treatment in order to increase the efficacy for both the 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 21 disorders and at the same time reduce the medication intake. In addition, safety might be difficult to manage due to drug-drug interactions [49]. However, a significant proportion of CM patients needing prevention do not receive it [50], and even when it is prescribed, there is a high rate of nonadherence (approximately 35–50%) [39]. Low adherence to preventive drugs is mainly due to relatively low and inconstant efficacy and to bothersome side effects [5, 8, 38]. 2.13 Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Headache Management Placebo is defined as a physiologically inactive substance that elicits a therapeutic response by inducing changes in symptoms or conditions [28]. The counterpart of placebo is nocebo. The term refers to unpleasant or undesirable effects (e.g., adverse events) that occur after the administration of placebo [28]. The therapeutic effect of any intervention, both pharmacological or not, is the result of the efficacy of the intervention and the placebo effect. Similarly, the tolerability of medications depends on adverse events and nocebo. Nocebo adversely influences quality of life and is a significant cofactor for treatment adherence and failure [37]. While nocebo effect is a nuisance in both clinical research and clinical practice, placebo effect has different meanings for doctors and scientists. In clinical research the placebo effect is the enemy that needs to be neutralized in order to properly demonstrate the benefits of active medications [61]. At the opposite, in clinical practice the placebo effect is the useful friend that provides additional efficacy or further increasing tolerability [9, 14]. Accordingly, doctors should try to maximize the placebo effect while minimizing the nocebo effect. The placebo responsiveness seems related to some psychological traits (e.g., somatic focus, hypnotic suggestibility, dispositional optimism, empathy) [25, 33, 34, 42, 43, 63] and personality traits (e.g., novelty seeking, behavioral drive, harm avoidance, reward responsiveness) [70]. Previous experience influences future outcomes, and previous response to pain therapy influences future analgesia. For example, patients with chronic neuropathic pain have a different magnitude of placebo analgesia depending on prior exposure to either successful or unsuccessful treatment [1]. Placebo analgesia can occur also without direct first-hand experience, for example, in patients who have observed a benefit in another person [19]. Interpersonal interactions can play a fundamental role in placebo effects. Empathy, the ability to empathize another’s feelings, may facilitate these effects [19], and interacting with the physician can trigger the mechanisms underlying placebo analgesia with relevant clinical results [3, 22, 59]. Nice doctor’s manners, a clear and empathic language, a clean and comfortable environment, and an easy access to doctors and care are just some of the factors that contribute to a placebo effect and that physicians should always take in great consideration. In general, if placebo effects act as reinforcers of clinical outcomes, nocebo effects may be responsible for drug intolerance and treatment failure [20]. Nocebo 22 A. Negro and P. Martelletti effects encompass both symptoms that resemble those expected of the active drug and nonspecific adverse events that cannot be explained by the pharmacologic action of a drug [2]. This reaction originates from patient’s negative expectation that a medical treatment will most likely produce harmful consequences instead of helping [60]. Some subjects could be more suitable to develop nocebo in presence of comorbidity with anxiety and depressive disorders. Among other cofactors, patient’s prior conditioning and suggestions, anxiety modulation, and previous repetitive treatment failures or discontinuation due to adverse events generate negative beliefs for the treatment outcome and safety, inducing nocebo. As placebo is induced by pretrial positive suggestions [21, 30, 78], there is evidence that nocebo is verbally induced by investigators with pretrial negative suggestions [23]. Similarly, in clinical practice detailed and extensive information on potential side effects by physicians can also trigger nocebo adverse events [18]. The proper delivery of drug safety information to patients is a crucial step that can avoid nocebo generated by reading the drug brochures and nowadays the Internet information on drug safety. Definitively, the content and the way information are presented to patients are fundamental to minimize in clinical trials both placebo and nocebo effects and in clinical practice to increase placebo while neutralizing nocebo effects. References 1. Andre-Obadia N, Magnin M, Garcia-Larrea L. On the importance of placebo timing in rTMS studies for pain relief. Pain. 2011;152:1233–7. 2. Barsky AJ, Saintfort R, Rogers MP, et al. Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon. JAMA. 2002;287:622–7. 3. Benedetti F. Placebo and the new physiology of the doctor-patient relationship. Physiol Rev. 2013;93:1207–46. 4. Bigal ME, Serrano D, Buse D, et al. Acute migraine medications and evolution from episodic to chronic migraine: a longitudinal population-based study. Headache. 2008;48:1157–68. 5. Bigal ME, Serrano D, Reed M, et al. Chronic migraine in the population: burden, diagnosis, and satisfaction with treatment. Neurology. 2008;71:559–66. 6. Bigal ME, Kurth T, Santanello N, et al. Migraine and cardiovascular disease: a populationbased study. Neurology. 2010;74:628–35. 7. Bigal ME, Buse DC, Chen YT, et al. Rates and predictors of starting a triptan: results from the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention Study. Headache. 2010;50:1440–8. 8. Blumenfeld AM, Bloudek LM, Becker WJ, et al. Patterns of use and reasons for discontinuation of prophylactic medications for episodic migraine and chronic migraine: results from the second International Burden of Migraine Study (IBMS-II). Headache. 2013;53:644–55. 9. Bostick NA, Sade R, Levine MA, et al. Placebo use in clinical practice: report of the American Medical Association Council on Ethical and Judicial Affairs. J Clin Ethics. 2008;19:58–61. 10. Buse DC, Bigal ME, Serrano D, et al. Triptan use patterns among migraine sufferers: results of the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention Study (AMPP). Cephalalgia. 2009;29:11. 11. Buse DC, Serrano D, Reed ML, et al. Adding additional acute medications to a triptan regimen for migraine and observed changes in headache-related disability: results from the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) Study. Headache. 2015;55:825–39. 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 23 12. Cady RK, Lipton RB, Hall C, et al. Treatment of mild headache in disabled migraine sufferers: results of the Spectrum Study. Headache. 2000;40:792–7. 13. Cady RK, Sheftell F, Lipton RB, et al. Effect of early intervention with sumatriptan on migraine pain: retrospective analyses of data from three clinical trials. Clin Ther. 2000;22:1035–48. 14. Cahana A, Romagnioli S. Not all placebos are the same: a debate on the ethics of placebo use in clinical trials versus clinical practice. J Anesth. 2007;21:102–5. 15. Charles C, Whelan T, Gafni A. What do we mean by partnership in making decisions about treatment? BMJ. 1999;319:780–2. 16. Cho SJ, Song TJ, Chu MK. Treatment update of chronic migraine. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2017;21:26. 17. Chu MK, Buse DC, Bigal ME, et al. Factors associated with triptan use in episodic migraine: results from the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention Study. Headache. 2012;52:213–23. 18. Cohen S. The nocebo effect of informed consent. Bioethics. 2014;28:147–54. 19. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain. 2009;144:28–34. 20. Colloca L, Grillon C. Understanding placebo and nocebo responses for pain management. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2014;18:419. 21. Colloca L, Miller FG. How placebo responses are formed: a learning perspective. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1859–69. 22. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, et al. Overt vs covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:679–84. 23. Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. Pain. 2008;136:211–8. 24. Davis KH, Black L, Sleath B. Validation of the patient perception of migraine questionnaire. Value Health. 2002;5:421–9. 25. De Pascalis V, Chiaradia C, Carotenuto E. The contribution of suggestibility and expectation to placebo analgesia phenomenon in an experimental setting. Pain. 2002;96:393–402. 26. Di Blasi Z, Harkness E, Ernst E, et al. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2001;357:757–62. 27. Dodick DW, Loder EW, Manack Adams A, et al. Assessing barriers to chronic migraine consultation, diagnosis, and treatment: results from the Chronic Migraine Epidemiology and Outcomes (CaMEO) Study. Headache. 2016;56(5):821–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/ head.12774. 28. Enck P, Benedetti F, Schedlowski M. New insights into the placebo and nocebo responses. Neuron. 2008;59:195–206. 29. Ferrari MD, Goadsby PJ, Roon KI, et al. Triptans (serotonin, 5-HT1B/1D agonists) in migraine: detailed results and methods of a meta-analysis of 53 trials. Cephalalgia. 2002;22:633–58. 30. Fiorio M, Recchia S, Corrà F, et al. Enhancing non-noxious perception: behavioural and neurophysiological correlates of a placebo-like manipulation. Neuroscience. 2012;217:96–104. 31. Foley KA, Cady R, Martin V, et al. Treating early versus treating mild: timing of migraine prescription medications among patients with diagnosed migraine. Headache. 2005;45:538–45. 32. Freund J, Krupp G, Goodenough D, et al. The doctor-patient relationship and drug effect. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1972;13:172–80. 33. Geers AL, Helfer SG, Kosbab K, et al. Reconsidering the role of personality in placebo effects: dispositional optimism, situational expectations, and the placebo response. J Psychosom Res. 2005;58:121–7. 34. Geers AL, Wellman JA, Fowler SL, et al. Dispositional optimism predicts placebo analgesia. J Pain. 2010;11:1165–71. 35. Griffin SJ, Kinmonth AL, Veltman MW, et al. Effect on health-related outcomes of interventions to alter the interaction between patients and practitioners: a systematic review of trials. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:595–608. 36. Gryll SL, Katahn M. Situational factors contributing to the placebos effect. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1978;57:253–61. 24 A. Negro and P. Martelletti 37. Heller MK, Chapman SC, Horne R. Beliefs about medication predict the misattribution of a common symptom as a medication side effect: evidence from an analogue online study. J Psychosom Res. 2015;79:519–29. 38. Hepp Z, Bloudek LM, Varon SF, et al. Systematic review of migraine prophylaxis adherence and persistence. J Manag Care Pharm. 2014;20:22–33. 39. Hepp Z, Dodick DW, Varon SF, et al. Adherence to oral migraine-preventive medications among patients with chronic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2015;35:478–88. 40. Holland S, Fanning KM, Serrano D, et al. Rates and reasons for discontinuation of triptans and opioids in episodic migraine: results from the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) study. J Neurol Sci. 2013;326:10–7. 41. Hu XH, Raskin NH, Cowan R, et al. Treatment of migraine with rizatriptan: when to take the medication. Headache. 2002;42:16–20. 42. Huber A, Lui F, Porro CA. Hypnotic susceptibility modulates brain activity related to experimental placebo analgesia. Pain. 2013;154:1509–18. 43. Johnston NE, Atlas LY, Wager TD. Opposing effects of expectancy and somatic focus on pain. PLoS One. 2012;7:e38854. 44. Kerse N, Buetow S, Mainous AG 3rd, et al. Physician-patient relationship and medication compliance: a primary care investigation. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:455–61. 45. Kurth T, Kase CS, Schürks M, et al. Migraine and risk of haemorrhagic stroke in women: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2010;341:c3659. 46. Landy SH, Runken MC, Bell CF, et al. Examining the interrelationship of migraine onset, duration, and time to treatment. Headache. 2012;52:363–73. 47. Landy SH, Turner IM, Runken MC, et al. A cross-sectional survey to assess the migraineur’s medication decision-making beliefs: determining when a migraine is triptan-worthy. Headache. 2013;53:1134–46. 48. Lionetto L, Negro A, Palmisani S, et al. Emerging treatment for chronic migraine and refractory chronic migraine. Expert Opin Emerg Drugs. 2012;17:393–406. 49. Lionetto L, Borro M, Curto M, et al. Choosing the safest acute therapy during chronic migraine prophylactic treatment: pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic considerations. Expert Opin Drug Metab Toxicol. 2016;12:399–406. 50. Lipton RB, Bigal ME, Diamond M, et al. Migraine prevalence, disease burden, and the need for preventive therapy. Neurology. 2007;68:343–9. 51. Lipton RB, Buse DC, Serrano D, et al. Acute medication use patterns in episodic migraine: results of the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention Study (AMPP). Presented at the 14th Congress of the International Headache Society, September 10–13, 2009. Cephalalgia. 2009;29:17. 52. Lipton RB, Serrano D, Holland S, et al. Barriers to the diagnosis and treatment of migraine: effects of sex, income, and headache features. Headache. 2013;53:81–92. 53. Lipton RB, Buse DC, Serrano D, et al. Examination of unmet treatment needs among persons with episodic migraine: results of the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) Study. Headache. 2013;53:1300–11. 54. Lipton RB, Fanning KM, Serrano D, et al. Ineffective acute treatment of episodic migraine is associated with new-onset chronic migraine. Neurology. 2015;84:688–95. 55. Lipton RB, Munjal S, Buse DC, et al. Predicting inadequate response to acute migraine medication: results from the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) Study. Headache. 2016;56:1635–48. 56. Mathew PG, Pavlovic JM, Lettich A, et al. Education and decision making at the time of triptan prescribing: patient expectations vs actual practice. Headache. 2014;54:698–708. 57. McDonald HP, Garg AX, Haynes RB. Interventions to enhance patient adherence to medication prescriptions: scientific review. JAMA. 2002;288:2868–79. 58. Messali AJ, Yang M, Gillard P, et al. Treatment persistence and switching in triptan users: a systematic literature review. Headache. 2014;54:1120–30. 2 Unmet Needs in Headache Management 25 59. Miller FG, Colloca L, Kaptchuk TJ. The placebo effect: illness and interpersonal healing. Perspect Biol Med. 2009;52:518–39. 60. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headache. Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;29:331–6. 61. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis LI, Chalarakis NG. Nocebo is the enemy, not placebo. A meta-analysis of reported side effects after placebo treatment in headaches. Cephalalgia. 2011;31:550–61. 62. Mitsikostas DD, Edvinsson L, Jensen RH, et al. Refractory chronic cluster headache: a consensus statement on clinical definition from the European Headache Federation. J Headache Pain. 2014;15:79. 63. Morton DL, Watson A, El-DeredyW, et al. Reproducibility of placebo analgesia: effect of dispositional optimism. Pain. 2009;146:194–8. 64. Negro A, D’Alonzo L, Martelletti P. Chronic migraine: comorbidities, risk factors, and rehabilitation. Intern Emerg Med. 2010;5(Suppl 1):S13–9. 65. Negro A, Rocchietti-March M, Fiorillo M, et al. Chronic migraine: current concepts and ongoing treatments. Eur Rev Med Pharmacol Sci. 2011;15:1401–20. 66. Negro A, Curto M, Lionetto L, et al. A critical evaluation on MOH current treatments. Curr Treat Options Neurol. 2017;19:32. 67. Pascual J, Cabarrocas X. Within-patient early versus delayed treatment of migraine attacks with almotriptan: the sooner the better. Headache. 2002;42:28–31. 68. Pellesi L, Benemei S, Favoni V, et al. Quality indicators in headache care: an implementation study in six Italian specialist-care centres. J Headache Pain. 2017;18:55. 69. Sacco S, Ricci S, Carolei A. Migraine and vascular diseases: a review of the evidence and potential implications for management. Cephalalgia. 2012;32:785–95. 70. Schweinhardt P, Seminowicz DA, Jaeger E, et al. The anatomy of the mesolimbic reward system: a link between personality and the placebo analgesic response. J Neurosci. 2009;29:4882–7. 71. Serrano D, Buse DC, Kori SH, et al. Effects of switching acute treatment on disability in migraine patients using triptans. Headache. 2013;53:1415–29. 72. Serrano D, Buse DC, Manack Adams A, et al. Acute treatment optimization in episodic and chronic migraine: results of the American Migraine Prevalence and Prevention (AMPP) Study. Headache. 2015;55:502–18. 73. Sheftell FD, Feleppa M, Tepper SJ, et al. Patterns of use of triptans and reasons for switching them in a tertiary care migraine population. Headache. 2004;44:661–8. 74. Stewart M. Continuity, care, and commitment: the course of patient-clinician relationships. Ann Fam Med. 2004;2:388–90. 75. Tfelt-Hansen P, Olesen J. Taking the negative view of current migraine treatments: the unmet needs. CNS Drugs. 2012;26:375–82. 76. Thomas KB. General practice consultations: is there any point in being positive. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1987;294:1200–2. 77. Turner JA, Deyo RA, Loeser JD, et al. The importance of placebo effects in pain treatment and research. JAMA. 1994;271:1609–14. 78. Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain. 1990;43:121–8. 79. Wells RE, Markowitz SY, Baron EP, et al. Identifying the factors underlying discontinuation of triptans. Headache. 2014;54:278–89. Chapter 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache Alexandre F. M. DaSilva and Marcos Fabio DosSantos 3.1 Introduction Pain is a highly disabling and prevalent symptom present in many clinical conditions [37]. It can be classified into acute and chronic, an extremely simplistic terminology, but with distinctive importance for the patient’s daily life and therapeutic decisions in the clinical practice [9, 35, 36, 87]. Although still a puzzle, understanding of the pain phenomenon and mechanisms has considerably evolved throughout the last decades, leading to a broader concept of pain “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage” [66]. In fact, such designation has its bases intrinsically linked to the seminal work published by Melzack and Casey [65]. Since then, it has been largely accepted that pain cannot be restricted to nociception, simply confined to the interpretation of nature, intensity, duration, and location of noxious stimuli. Instead, pain goes beyond the discriminative evaluation of noxious stimuli and comprises emotional (motivational) and cognitive (evaluative) neural components. Therefore, the contribution of not only sensory but also limbic and higher-order A. F. M. DaSilva (*) Headache and Orofacial Pain Effort (H.O.P.E.), Department of Biologic and Materials Sciences, School of Dentistry, The Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience Institute (MBNI), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA e-mail: [email protected] M. F. DosSantos Headache and Orofacial Pain Effort (H.O.P.E.), Department of Biologic and Materials Sciences, School of Dentistry, The Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience Institute (MBNI), University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Laboratório de Morfogênese Celular (LMC), Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas (ICB), Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_3 27 28 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos brain structures as well as facilitatory and inhibitory pain pathways to this phenomenon has been explored and largely accepted [65]. Another important concept that must be considered is that rather than differentiating acute and chronic pain only based on chronological marks (e.g., 3 or 6 months), the differential participation of etiological factors and the more important differences in their pathophysiologies must be fully understood. In fact, the mechanisms related to chronic pain have been partially unveiled in the recent years, and the uncreditable development of novel neuroimaging techniques has allowed the translational of the results obtained in experimental animal models of acute and chronic pain to translational clinical pain research in humans. In this respect, several mechanisms ranging from peripheral and central sensitization to maladaptive neuroplasticity (structural, functional, and molecular) have been investigated [6]. Nevertheless, it is important to establish that chronic pain is not considered a clinical diagnosis. Instead, it embraces several conditions that can be classified as neuropathic (trigeminal and postherpetic neuralgias, burning mouth syndrome, or diabetic neuropathy) or nociceptive (e.g., osteoarthritis, non-neuropathic low back pain), each group with its characteristic symptom profile [34, 47], which strongly suggest the presence of specific physiological mechanism mediating each chronic pain condition [77]. Therefore, the term “chronic pain syndromes” seems to be more appropriate to designate such conditions. Hence, the understanding of common as well as individual mechanisms might contribute to the development of less empirical and more precise therapies for each chronic pain syndrome, with significant lower side effects [45]. 3.2 unctional and Molecular Neuroplasticity F in Chronic Pain Advances in neuroimaging methods have allowed the identification of the main brain structures related to pain processing and modulation. Those areas include the primary (S1) and secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (rACC and dACC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), insula, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), thalamus, hypothalamus, amygdala, brainstem structures, and the main components of the reward circuitry such as nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral tegmental area (VTA), among others [60]. Several functional studies have demonstrated the presence of structural and functional neuroplastic changes in chronic pain syndromes. One of the classic studies in the field found decreased gray matter density in the bilateral DLPFC and right thalamus in chronic back pain, with distinct characteristics in neuropathic and non-neuropathic patients [3]. In addition, the amygdala volume as well as the density of the white matter connections within the corticolimbic system have been considered risk factors to the development of chronic pain [86]. Colocalization between structural changes and oscillations in the functional brain activity related to pain have also been explored through functional magnetic 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 29 resonance imaging (fMRI). One of the first studies revealed that functional activations induced by cutaneous allodynia in the maxillary territory of the trigeminal nerve (V2) are colocalized with cortical thickening/thinning in sensorimotor areas or cortical thinning in brain areas related to the emotional processing of pain in trigeminal neuropathic pain patients [23]. Using an original protocol, Baliki and collaborators used fMRI to demonstrate that the activity of the NAc during the offset of painful thermal stimulus as well as its connectivity with the PFC and insula permits to differentiate chronic back at a very high accuracy chronic pain patients and healthy subjects [4]. Such pattern of NAc activation has been considered a possible biomarker of chronic pain [94]. A further studied demonstrated that higher connectivity between the NAc and PFC could also predict persistent pain. Based on such findings, it has been theorized that corticostriatal projections are very likely major elements in pain chronification [5]. It is important to consider that the input of the nociceptive information to the central nervous system (CNS) is under direct control of complex endogenous modulatory systems. Those systems act through several different neurotransmitters, including glutamate, gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), endogenous opioids, dopamine, and serotonin, among others, and regulate the flow of information to cortical regions of the CNS, acting mainly at the level of the dorsal horn of the spinal cord [64]. They also act through a descending pain inhibitory system, composed by several brainstem structures mainly the locus coeruleus, periaqueductal grey matter (PAG), raphe nuclei and rostroventromedial medulla (RVM). The involvement of the thalamus, hypothalamus, and cortical areas, including the insula, amygdala, and cingulate cortex, has also been shown [46]. However, PAG, RMV, and spinal dorsal horn have been described as the principal structures in the descending pain modulation [58]. PAG and RVM act mainly through serotonin (5-HT) receptors [2]. More recently the contribution of endocannabinoid receptors CB1 to this inhibitory process has been suggested [32]. Moreover, those structures seem to be important to the analgesia related to several classes of drugs including antidepressants, nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory, and opioids [70]. Nonetheless, scarce information is available regarding the specific anatomical structures and pathways involved in the endogenous pain modulation. In this scenario, novel studies have explored the in vivo activation of chief modulatory systems (e.g., opioidergic and dopaminergic) through positron-emission tomography (PET) related to both acute experimental condition [95] and chronic pain syndromes [24, 26, 33, 57, 68]. The first studies investigating the presence of changes in the opioid neurotransmission related to chronic pain measured the nondisplacable binding potential (BPND) of the nonselective opioid receptor radiotracer [11C] diprenorphine and found a decrease in [11C] diprenorphine BPND in such patients [48, 89]. Further studies started to evaluate the same parameter of the selective mu-opioid receptor (MOR) radioligand, [11C] carfentanil. One of those studies explored functional changes in the mu-opioid system in patients with a fibromyalgia syndrome, a highly prevalent chronic pain condition with still unknown pathophysiology wherein opioid analgesic has low effectiveness. In fact, the results of that study were considered 30 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos an initial evidence that a dysfunction of the mu-opioid system may play a role in fibromyalgia. More specifically, that study indicated that fibromyalgia patients have lower availability of MORs in several brain areas related to pain, including dorsal dACC, NAc, and amygdala. Moreover, a negative correlation between the MOR BPND within the NAc and the clinical pain scores was demonstrated, which corroborates the hypothesis of the involvement of a dysfunctional opioid and particularly mu-opioid neurotransmission in that syndrome [43]. Another clinical neuroimaging study conducted in fibromyalgia patients found a positive correlation between the pain-evoked brain activity, measured through fMRI and MOR availability in the rACC and in the DLPFC as well as a negative correlation between both BOLD (blood oxygen level dependent) signals related to pain and MOR BPND and the affective/sensory pain ratio in the medial frontal gyrus (MFG) and in the CC [73]. A mechanism involving lower affinity or downregulation of MORs on GABAergic interneurons located in the PFC and anterior cingulate cortex caused by tonic higher levels of endogenous opioids has been suggested based on such results [73]. Therefore, the inhibition of GABAergic interneurons produced by phasic release of endogenous opioids associated with noxious stimulation would be impaired in fibromyalgia syndrome, leading to a faulty descending pain modulation that would explain the persistent pain present in those patients. PET studies have also demonstrated altered dopaminergic functioning driven by chronic pain, and results suggest that specific mechanisms can occur in each group of chronic pain disorder. For example, while a decreased availability of dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the ventral striatum has been found in fibromyalgia patients [90] and chronic back pain [57], an increased availability of dopamine receptors has been demonstrated in chronic orofacial neuropathic disorders such as burning mouth syndrome (BMS) and atypical facial pain (AFP) [41, 42]. The reduced availability of dopamine D2/D3 receptors found chronic back pain patients was negatively correlated to the pain intensity [57]. Furthermore, a coupling between MOR and D2/D3R in the amygdala was related to experimental pain. To which extension this coupling could between the activity of the dopaminergic and opioidergic systems contribute to the mechanisms of chronic pain, analgesia, and opioid dependence remains to be investigated [57]. 3.3 unctional and Structural Neuroplasticity in Headaches F and Migraine While pain has been attributed as a major factor that conduces patients to seek medical advice [37], headache has been referred as the most prevalent symptom in patients that seek medical care. In addition, primary headache disorders are the most frequent brain disorders [83]. Among them, tension-type headache (TTH) and migraine are the predominant subtypes of primary headaches [82]. Migraine lifetime prevalence has been estimated in 14% [84], while its 1-year prevalence is around 35%, which is very close to the 38% estimated for TTH [82]. Trigeminal 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 31 autonomic cephalalgias (TACs), including cluster headache, paroxysmal hemicranias, hemicrania continua, short-lasting unilateral neuralgiform headache attacks with conjunctival injection and tearing (SUNCT), and short-lasting unilateral neuralgiform headache attacks with cranial autonomic symptoms (SUNA), constitute an additional class of primary headaches. Those headaches are characterized by the occurrence of autonomic signal symptoms that take place ipsilateral to the pain. They all share some pathophysiological features. Nonetheless, they differ in many clinical aspects such as duration and frequency of the headache episodes and response to therapy [17]. As previously mentioned, migraine is a very prevalent neurological disorder. It is also markedly more common in women than in men [51]. Migraine is characterized by episodes of unilateral pulsating headache, associated with nausea and/or phonophobia and photophobia. Sensitivity to touch and odor may also occur. Two migraine subtypes have been described, migraine without aura (MwoA) and migraine with aura (MwA). MwA is defined by the presence of related visual or sensory symptoms concurrent to or anticipating a migraine episode [28, 44]. Some individuals evolve from episodic to chronic migraine, which is characterized by a headache that happening on 15 or more days per month for a period greater than 3 months, and that features the common aspects of a migraine headache on at least 8 days per month [44]. Nonetheless, more important than the number of episodes is the occurrence of specific symptoms related to chronic migraine, including hyperalgesia and allodynia, both highly prevalent in migraineurs [8, 11, 12, 52, 54] and directly linked to neuroplastic phenomena, including central and peripheral sensitization [10, 31]. In fact, it has also been reported that around two-thirds of migraineurs experience allodynia during a migraine attack [12]. Its incidence seems to be more frequent in chronic migraine patients. In addition, it has been shown that the duration of the disease might affect not only the occurrence but also the severity of those symptoms [7]. Remarkably, a decreased connectivity between the PAG, ACC, and PFC has also been found in migraine patients with allodynia when compared to migraine patients without allodynia [55]. Several studies have shown the presence of changes in the structure and function of patients with migraine headache [20, 25, 38, 39, 49, 55]. Those studies were important to demonstrate that rather than affecting specific brain areas and neural circuits, migraine is associated with changes in a broad set of brain structures and connected networks, which helps to explain the variety of clinical symptoms that constitute this disorder [16, 17]. One pioneer research in this area investigated the mechanisms of MwA using fMRI [39]. The study showed a focal increase in the BOLD signal within the extrastriate cortex (V3A), progressing slowly and contiguously over the occipital cortex following a retinotopic visual organization, during a migraine headache attack with visual aura. Interestingly, this initial increase was followed a reduction of the BOLD signal following the same retinotopic progression. Those events suggest the occurrence of vasodilatation (seen through an increase in the BOLD signal) followed by a vasoconstriction (as observed by a decreased in the BOLD signal) and the presence of cortical spreading depression in the human visual cortex of migraineurs [39]. A following study also found structural 32 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos changes, represented by increased cortical thickness in the V3A area and also MT+, both involved in motion processing, in patients with MwA and MwoA [38]. Increased thickness was further demonstrated in the somatosensory cortex of migraineurs, with most prominent changes discovered in the cortical area that contains a somatotopic representation of the head and face [21]. However, contradictory findings such as the lack of changes in the cortical thickness [27] and even cortical thinning have also been reported in migraine patients [15], when compared to healthy subjects. The results of one of those studies indicated that migraine patients present age-related thinning of some cortical regions that do not thin in healthy controls and that only exhibit more pronounce thinning with advancing age. Such findings led the authors to suggest that migraine interacts with aging to produce structural cortical changes. More recently, a growing number of studies have also explored the occurrence of changes in the functional connectivity (fc) of the brain at rest (evaluated through rs-fMRI) in headaches. However, such findings should be considered cautiously due to the presence of methodological limitations [74]. Studies that investigated changes related to the ictal phase of migraine headaches revealed decreased fc between the executive and dorsoventral networks in migraine patients without aura, compared to healthy controls [18], and increased fc between the insula (bilateral) and the mPFC and between the mPFC and the PCC [19]. Moreover, the pain intensity during migraine attacks negatively correlated to the strength of the mPFC-insula connectivity [19]. Another study performed in patients with migraine with aura found an increased fc between the dorsolateral pons and the ipsilateral somatosensory cortex (head and face somatotopic areas). Such results reinforce the concept of a migraine “generator,” located in dorsolateral pons, a designation that comes from a classic PET study that proposed a mechanism for migraine based on an imbalance in the activity between the vascular control and the brainstem antinociceptive regulation [88]. As a matter of fact, the brainstem seems to play a crucial role in the migraine and headaches pathophysiology, especially the dorsal ponto-mesencephalic junction, the dorsal pons, and the trigeminal nuclei [13, 55, 67, 80, 81, 88]. For instance, it has been demonstrated that the specificity of triptan to treat headaches and migraine and not pain in general might be explained by a functional inhibition of trigeminal-cortical projections [50]. Furthermore, a stronger connectivity between the PAG and areas related to the somatosensory processing and nociception has been reported in migraine patients when compared to healthy subjects [55]. These results are in accordance with the findings of previous studies that had reported lower functional anisotropy in the ventrolateral PAG in MwoA patients during interictal periods [22]. Nevertheless, although there is enough scientific evidence regarding the important role of the brainstem activity to migraine headache, the presence of a “migraine generator” has been recently revisited. The results of one study that analyzed the data of a migraine patient that underwent MRI exams every day for 30 days revealed an increase in the hypothalamus activity 24 h prior to the pain onset (e.g., increased hypothalamic activity toward a next migraine attack). In addition, the same study showed changes in the coupling between the hypothalamus and the two areas of the 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 33 brainstem: the spinal trigeminal nuclei and the “migraine generator” region [75]. Therefore, instead of a “migraine generator,” the connectivity between the hypothalamus and the brainstem seems to be determinant to the development of migraine attacks. Furthermore, the role of spontaneous oscillations of complex networks related to the brainstem, hypothalamus, and dopaminergic networks altering the activity of brainstem and subcortical areas in the development of migraine headache has also been discussed [61]. Interestingly, an anteroposterior segmentation of the hypothalamic roles to different stages of migraine has been suggested. In this regard, it has been postulated that the posterior hypothalamus would have a more relevant participation in acute phases, while the anterior hypothalamus would be more important to the chronification process [76]. Posterior hypothalamus activation seems also to be a key element not only for migraine but also for TAC pathophysiology as demonstrated by several studies involving hemicrania continua [59], SUNCT [63, 79], and cluster headache [62, 78]. In addition, altered hypothalamic fc has been demonstrated in cluster headache patients [71, 92]. This abnormal hypothalamic activity and connectivity could explain some unique aspects seen in this group of headaches, including the circadian and circannual rhythmus and restless sensation [17]. However, how exactly these findings could explain the main signs and symptoms reported by TAC patients must be examined in-depth. When comparing the fc of MwA, MwoA, and healthy subjects, it is possible to find shared and specific mechanisms. For instance, a previous study found that migraine patients (with or without aura) had increased fc between the right temporal region and the middle frontal gyrus. Nonetheless, MwA but not MwoA presented reduced fc between the occipital cortex (V3A area) and the anterior insula, which was in turn correlated to the headache severity in MwA but not in MwoA patients [69]. Thus a contribution of the reduced fc observed in the occipital cortex of MwA patients to the development of visual aura has been suggested [69]. Nonetheless, an altered fc in extrastriate areas of the occipital cortex has also been raised as a potential biomarker of MwA, since, when evaluated in the interictal phase, MwA patients exhibited increased fc in lingual gyrus of the extrastriate cortex, which is involved in the initiation and propagation of the migraine aura, when compared to MwoA and healthy subjects. Moreover, such abnormal resting-state connectivity was neither correlated to the severity of migraine nor accompanied by structural changes which indicates the specificity of the results of the study [85]. Differences between MwoA and MwA have been also investigated through 1 H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), which permits a noninvasive estimative of central metabolite levels. One study that investigated oscillation in metabolite levels within the visual cortex related to photic stimulation assessed interictally reported a more pronounced reduction in the signal of NAA (N-acetylaspartate) in MwA patients when compared to MwoA and healthy individuals, which could be interpreted as an impaired mitochondrial functioning in MwA, since no differences were observed between MwoA and healthy individuals [72]. Some studies have also raised a possible mechanism involving different patterns of amygdala fc as a mechanism to the development of episodic and chronic migraine. 34 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos In one of those studies, an increased fc occurred between the amygdala and the secondary somatosensory cortex (S2) and anterior insula and thalamus in migraine patients but not in other painful disorders that area not putatively related to cortical spreading depression (CSD) such as trigeminal neuralgia and carpal tunnel syndrome. Such results have been interpreted as a dysfunction of a neurolimbic pain network driven by repeated episodes of CSD which in turn would contribute to the pathophysiology of migraine headaches [40]. A further study demonstrated differences in the amygdala fc (e.g., increased left amygdala in episodic migraine patients and decreased right amygdala fc in chronic migraine patients) when compared to healthy controls, supporting the involvement of the neurolimbic pain network to migraine mechanisms and possibly to migraine chronification [14]. Finally, the participation of resting-state connectivity abnormalities to migraine has also been evidenced by reduced regional homogeneity (ReHo) detected in the PFC, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), rACC, and supplementary motor area (SMA) found in MwoA patients compared to healthy subjects and by the higher connectivity between the default mode network (DMN) and central executive network (CEN) and the insula, which were correlated to the migraine duration in MwoA patients compared to controls [91]. Increased ReHo in the PCC/precuneus, pons, and trigeminal nerve entry zone and decreased fc between these areas with altered ReHo and other brain regions have also been found in patients with MwoA compared to healthy subjects. Based on the crucial role of the precuneus/PCC to the default mode network, which is important for several physiological functions including self-monitoring and interoception, it has been suggested that MwoA patients may have dysfunction involving information transfer and multimodal integration [93]. 3.4 olecular and Neuroplasticity and Metabolic Changes M in Headaches and Migraine Changes in specific neurochemistry and endogenous modulatory systems in headaches have been recently explored through positron-emission tomography (PET). Early scientific evidence of altered function of modulatory systems in headaches comes from studies investigating changes in the availability of serotonin receptors, 5-HT(1A) receptors associated with episodes of migraine headaches. When compared to healthy controls and headache-free migraineurs, migraine patients that developed a headache attack during the PET scan showed an increased BPND of the a selective 5-HT(1A) antagonist [(18)F]MPP in the pontine raphe [29]. This study brought insights the participation of the pontine raphe region and 5-HT(1A) receptors in the migraine pathophysiology. Increased MPP BPND has also been shown in limbic regions as well as in the posterior parieto-occipital and temporal cortical regions during interictal periods of MwoA patients, which could possibly reflect decreased serotonin levels or increased expression of 5-HT(1A) in the brain of those patients [53]. 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 35 In line with such studies, our group has recently demonstrated the occurrence of functional changes of mu-opioid neurotransmission associated with ictal trigeminal allodynia, in migraineurs [68], corroborating the concept that maladaptive neuroplastic changes contribute to the mechanisms of migraine headache. The first prelaminar PET study used an interactive 3D immersive approach to investigate the mu-opioid neurotransmission in vivo during a migraine attack. The results showed lower MORs BPND in the CC, PAG, NAc, and thalamus during the ictal phase of a migraine patient (Fig. 3.1). Such findings could represent higher occupancy of MORs by endogenous opioids released in response to the ongoing headache [24]. Recent studies have also demonstrated changes in the mu-opioid neurotransmission associated with migraine headache attacks (ictal phase) in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), a brain region previously associated with migraine [1, 30]. Moreover, another study found a positive correlation between migraine-related MOR activation in key brainstem structures for pain (e.g., red nucleus and PAG) and the concomitant occurrence of thermal allodynia in such patients [68]. Decrease in µ-Opioid Receptor Availability During a Migraine attack ACC Nac Ictal Interictal Thal Severe Moderate Mild 0 1 2 3 4 Fig. 3.1 Reduced MOR availability during a migraine headache attack in vivo. Lower panel represents a reduced nondisplacable binding potential (BPND) of the selective mu-opioid receptor radiotracer [11C] carfentanil during the ictal phase of a migraine headache, possibly reflecting a higher release of endogenous opioid in response to the ongoing pain, when compared to the interictal phase (upper panel). Thal (thalamus), Nac (nucleus accumbens), ACC (anterior cingulate cortex). J Vis Exp. 2014 Jun 2;(88) 36 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos Interictal phase (No headache) Presynaptic Migraine attack (Headache at rest) Postsynaptic Dopamine (D2/D3 (endogenous) (11C)raclopride D2/D3 receptor Cutaneous allodynia (Headache + STPT challenge) Decreased edogenous dopamine release relative to interictal phase Sudden increase in endogenous dopamine release relative to ongoing headache at rest (11C)raclopride BP Fig. 3.2 Imbalanced release of endogenous dopamine related to migraine headache and allodynia in vivo. Left panel shows steady levels of endogenous dopamine during the interictal phase of migraine. Center panel represents reduced striatal dopamine release during spontaneous migraine attacks compared to the interictal phase. Right panel shows an increased release of dopamine during cutaneous allodynia when compared to ongoing headache at rest. STPT sustained thermal pain threshold challenge; BP binding potential. Neurology. 2017 Apr 25;88(17):1634–41 Changes in the normal functioning of the dopaminergic system have only been recently shown in the brain of migraineurs in vivo [26], wherein a significant increased availability of dopamine D2/D3 receptors was found in the dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen nucleus) during the ictal migraine phase compared to the interictal period. Interestingly, a decrease in the dopamine D2/D3 receptors availability was driven by thermal allodynia, indicting a possible contribution of a dynamic fluctuation in dopamine levels to the migraine pathophysiology [26] (Fig. 3.2). Although both structural and functional changes had been previously observed in the striatum of migraineurs [56], the investigation of dynamic changes comparing ictal and interictal phases and induced by cutaneous thermal allodynia provides novel and significant information, since considering that migraine is a cyclic pain disorder a simple comparison between migraineurs at the acute phase and healthy subjects is no longer considered enough to explain the altered brain function in migraine patients. As previously stated, the simple comparison between migraine patients during the ictal phase and healthy subjects does not allow to separate the specific changes that are driven by a migraine headache attack from those which are really related to the pathophysiology of the disease [17]. Hence, study designs that include the evaluation of migraine patients at different phases of the migraine cycle and longitudinal studies have been considered ideal [74] for migraine research studies. 3.5 Concluding Remarks There is mounting evidence in the current literature demonstrating the occurrence of neuroplastic changes driven by chronic pain, including primary headaches and especially migraine. Most of the studies focused on the presence of structural, functional, and more recently connectivity changes in the brain of chronic pain patients. Some studies have also pointed toward the presence of potential biomarkers that 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 37 could help to differentiate chronic pain patients from health controls or indicate an ongoing chronification process. However, the translation of those findings to the clinical practice is still very limited. Recent studies have also proved the presence of an altered functioning of major endogenous modulatory systems (e.g., opioidergic and dopaminergic) in different chronic pain disorders. Only with more detailed information regarding the specific contribution of each of those systems, combined with a precise evaluation of the altered structure and function of pain-related brain structures in each chronic pain syndrome, will permit a complete understanding of aberrant and/or deficient processing and modulation of nociceptive information in chronic pain and headaches, which could then be applied in the tailoring of new therapeutic strategies for those patients. References 1. Afridi SK, Matharu MS, Lee L, Kaube H, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS, et al. A PET study exploring the laterality of brainstem activation in migraine using glyceryl trinitrate. Brain. 2005;128.(Pt 4:932–9. 2. Aimone LD, Jones SL, Gebhart GF. Stimulation-produced descending inhibition from the periaqueductal gray and nucleus raphe magnus in the rat: mediation by spinal monoamines but not opioids. Pain. 1987;31(1):123–36. 3. Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Sonty S, Levy RM, Harden RN, Parrish TB, et al. Chronic back pain is associated with decreased prefrontal and thalamic gray matter density. J Neurosci. 2004;24(46):10410–5. 4. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, Apkarian AV. Predicting value of pain and analgesia: nucleus accumbens response to noxious stimuli changes in the presence of chronic pain. Neuron. 2010;66(1):149–60. 5. Baliki MN, Petre B, Torbey S, Herrmann KM, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(8):1117–9. 6. Basbaum AI, Bautista DM, Scherrer G, Julius D. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of pain. Cell. 2009;139(2):267–84. 7. Benatto MT, Florencio LL, Carvalho GF, Dach F, Bigal ME, Chaves TC, et al. Cutaneous allodynia is more frequent in chronic migraine, and its presence and severity seems to be more associated with the duration of the disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2017;75(3):153–9. 8. Bigal ME, Ashina S, Burstein R, Reed ML, Buse D, Serrano D, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of allodynia in headache sufferers: a population study. Neurology. 2008;70(17):1525–33. 9. Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain. 2006;10(4):287–333. 10. Burstein R. Deconstructing migraine headache into peripheral and central sensitization. Pain. 2001;89(2–3):107–10. 11. Burstein R, Cutrer MF, Yarnitsky D. The development of cutaneous allodynia during a migraine attack clinical evidence for the sequential recruitment of spinal and supraspinal nociceptive neurons in migraine. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 8):1703–9. 12. Burstein R, Yarnitsky D, Goor-Aryeh I, Ransil BJ, Bajwa ZH. An association between migraine and cutaneous allodynia. Ann Neurol. 2000;47(5):614–24. 13. Cao Y, Aurora SK, Nagesh V, Patel SC, Welch KM. Functional MRI-BOLD of brainstem structures during visually triggered migraine. Neurology. 2002;59(1):72–8. 14. Chen Z, Chen X, Liu M, Dong Z, Ma L, Yu S. Altered functional connectivity of amygdala underlying the neuromechanism of migraine pathogenesis. J Headache Pain. 2017;18(1):7. 38 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos 15. Chong CD, Dodick DW, Schlaggar BL, Schwedt TJ. Atypical age-related cortical thinning in episodic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2014;34(14):1115–24. 16. Chong CD, Schwedt TJ, Dodick DW. Migraine: what imaging reveals. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2016;16(7):64. 17. Chong CD, Schwedt TJ, Hougaard A. Brain functional connectivity in headache disorders: a narrative review of MRI investigations. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2017:271678X17740794. 18. Coppola G, Di Renzo A, Tinelli E, Di Lorenzo C, Di Lorenzo G, Parisi V, et al. Thalamo-cortical network activity during spontaneous migraine attacks. Neurology. 2016;87(20):2154–60. 19. Coppola G, Di Renzo A, Tinelli E, Di Lorenzo C, Scapeccia M, Parisi V, et al. Resting state connectivity between default mode network and insula encodes acute migraine headache. Cephalalgia 2017:333102417715230. 20. DaSilva AF, Granziera C, Snyder J, Hadjikhani N. Thickening in the somatosensory cortex of patients with migraine. Neurology. 2007;69(21):1990–5. 21. DaSilva A, Granziera C, Snyder J, Hadjikhani N. Thickening in the somatosensory cortex of patients with migraine. Neurology. 2007;69(21):1990–5. 22. DaSilva A, Granziera C, Tuch D, Snyder J, Vincent M, Hadjikhani N. Interictal alterations of the trigeminal somatosensory pathway and periaqueductal gray matter in migraine. Neuroreport. 2007;18(4):301–5. 23. DaSilva AF, Becerra L, Pendse G, Chizh B, Tully S, Borsook D. Colocalized structural and functional changes in the cortex of patients with trigeminal neuropathic pain. PLoS One. 2008;3(10):e3396. 24. DaSilva AF, Nascimento TD, Love T, DosSantos MF, Martikainen IK, Cummiford CM, et al. 3D-neuronavigation in vivo through a patient’s brain during a spontaneous migraine headache. J Vis Exp. 2014;(88). 25. DaSilva AF, Nascimento TD, DosSantos MF, Lucas S, van HolsbeecK H, DeBoer M, et al. Association of μ-opioid activation in the prefrontal cortex with spontaneous migraine attacks— brief report I. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2014;1(6):439–44. 26. DaSilva AF, Nascimento TD, Jassar H, Heffernan J, Toback RL, Lucas S, et al. Dopamine D2/ D3 imbalance during migraine attack and allodynia in vivo. Neurology. 2017;88(17):1634–41. 27. Datta R, Detre JA, Aguirre GK, Cucchiara B. Absence of changes in cortical thickness in patients with migraine. Cephalalgia. 2011;31(14):1452–8. 28. Demarquay G, Royet JP, Giraud P, Chazot G, Valade D, Ryvlin P. Rating of olfactory judgements in migraine patients. Cephalalgia. 2006;26(9):1123–30. 29. Demarquay G, Lothe A, Royet JP, Costes N, Mick G, Mauguière F, et al. Brainstem changes in 5-HT1A receptor availability during migraine attack. Cephalalgia. 2011;31(1):84–94. 30. Denuelle M, Fabre N, Payoux P, Chollet F, Geraud G. Hypothalamic activation in spontaneous migraine attacks. Headache. 2007;47(10):1418–26. 31. Dodick D, Silberstein S. Central sensitization theory of migraine: clinical implications. Headache. 2006;46(Suppl 4):S182–91. 32. Dogrul A, Seyrek M, Yalcin B, Ulugol A. Involvement of descending serotonergic and noradrenergic pathways in CB1 receptor-mediated antinociception. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2012;38(1):97–105. 33. Dossantos MF, Martikainen IK, Nascimento TD, Love TM, Deboer MD, Maslowski EC, et al. Reduced basal ganglia mu-opioid receptor availability in trigeminal neuropathic pain: a pilot study. Mol Pain. 2012;8(1):74. 34. Dworkin RH, Jensen MP, Gammaitoni AR, Olaleye DO, Galer BS. Symptom profiles differ in patients with neuropathic versus non-neuropathic pain. J Pain. 2007;8(2):118–26. 35. Elliott AM, Smith BH, Penny KI, Smith WC, Chambers WA. The epidemiology of chronic pain in the community. Lancet. 1999;354(9186):1248–52. 36. Elliott AM, Smith BH, Hannaford PC, Smith WC, Chambers WA. The course of chronic pain in the community: results of a 4-year follow-up study. Pain. 2002;99(1–2):299–307. 37. Fishman S, Ballantyne J, Rathmell JP, Bonica JJ. Bonica’s management of pain. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2010. 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 39 38. Granziera C, DaSilva AF, Snyder J, Tuch DS, Hadjikhani N. Anatomical alterations of the visual motion processing network in migraine with and without aura. PLoS Med. 2006;3(10):e402. 39. Hadjikhani N, Sanchez Del Rio M, Wu O, Schwartz D, Bakker D, Fischl B, et al. Mechanisms of migraine aura revealed by functional MRI in human visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(8):4687–92. 40. Hadjikhani N, Ward N, Boshyan J, Napadow V, Maeda Y, Truini A, et al. The missing link: enhanced functional connectivity between amygdala and visceroceptive cortex in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2013;33(15):1264–8. 41. Hagelberg N, Forssell H, Aalto S, Rinne JO, Scheinin H, Taiminen T, et al. Altered dopamine D2 receptor binding in atypical facial pain. Pain. 2003;106(1–2):43–8. 42. Hagelberg N, Forssell H, Rinne JO, Scheinin H, Taiminen T, Aalto S, et al. Striatal dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in burning mouth syndrome. Pain. 2003;101(1–2):149–54. 43. Harris RE, Clauw DJ, Scott DJ, McLean SA, Gracely RH, Zubieta JK. Decreased central muopioid receptor availability in fibromyalgia. J Neurosci. 2007;27(37):10000–6. 44. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS). The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition (beta version). Cephalalgia. 2013;33(9):629–808. 45. von Hehn CA, Baron R, Woolf CJ. Deconstructing the neuropathic pain phenotype to reveal neural mechanisms. Neuron. 2012;73(4):638–52. 46. Heinricher MM, Tavares I, Leith JL, Lumb BM. Descending control of nociception: specificity, recruitment and plasticity. Brain Res Rev. 2009;60(1):214–25. 47. Jensen MP, Dworkin RH, Gammaitoni AR, Olaleye DO, Oleka N, Galer BS. Assessment of pain quality in chronic neuropathic and nociceptive pain clinical trials with the Neuropathic Pain Scale. J Pain. 2005;6(2):98–106. 48. Jones AK, Cunningham VJ, Ha-Kawa S, Fujiwara T, Luthra SK, Silva S, et al. Changes in central opioid receptor binding in relation to inflammation and pain in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Br J Rheumatol. 1994;33(10):909–16. 49. Kim J, Suh S, Seol H, Oh K, Seo W, Yu S, et al. Regional grey matter changes in patients with migraine: a voxel-based morphometry study. Cephalalgia. 2008;28(6):598–604. 50. Kröger IL, May A. Triptan-induced disruption of trigemino-cortical connectivity. Neurology. 2015;84(21):2124–31. 51. Lipton RB, Bigal ME, Diamond M, Freitag F, Reed ML, Stewart WF, et al. Migraine prevalence, disease burden, and the need for preventive therapy. Neurology. 2007;68(5):343–9. 52. Lipton RB, Bigal ME, Ashina S, Burstein R, Silberstein S, Reed ML, et al. Cutaneous allodynia in the migraine population. Ann Neurol. 2008;63(2):148–58. 53. Lothe A, Merlet I, Demarquay G, Costes N, Ryvlin P, Mauguière F. Interictal brain 5-HT1A receptors binding in migraine without aura: a 18F-MPPF-PET study. Cephalalgia. 2008;28(12):1282–91. 54. Lovati C, D’Amico D, Bertora P. Allodynia in migraine: frequent random association or unavoidable consequence? Expert Rev Neurother. 2009;9(3):395–408. 55. Mainero C, Boshyan J, Hadjikhani N. Altered functional magnetic resonance imaging resting-state connectivity in periaqueductal gray networks in migraine. Ann Neurol. 2011;70(5):838–45. 56. Maleki N, Becerra L, Nutile L, Pendse G, Brawn J, Bigal M, et al. Migraine attacks the Basal Ganglia. Mol Pain. 2011;7:71. 57. Martikainen IK, Nuechterlein EB, Peciña M, Love TM, Cummiford CM, Green CR, et al. Chronic back pain is associated with alterations in dopamine neurotransmission in the ventral striatum. J Neurosci. 2015;35(27):9957–65. 58. Mason P. Ventromedial medulla: pain modulation and beyond. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493(1):2–8. 59. Matharu MS, Cohen AS, McGonigle DJ, Ward N, Frackowiak RS, Goadsby PJ. Posterior hypothalamic and brainstem activation in hemicrania continua. Headache. 2004;44(8):747–61. 60. May A. New insights into headache: an update on functional and structural imaging findings. Nat Rev Neurol. 2009;5(4):199–209. 40 A. F. M. DaSilva and M. F. DosSantos 61. May A. Understanding migraine as a cycling brain syndrome: reviewing the evidence from functional imaging. Neurol Sci. 2017;38(Suppl 1):125–30. 62. May A, Bahra A, Büchel C, Frackowiak RS, Goadsby PJ. Hypothalamic activation in cluster headache attacks. Lancet. 1998;352(9124):275–8. 63. May A, Bahra A, Büchel C, Turner R, Goadsby PJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging in spontaneous attacks of SUNCT: short-lasting neuralgiform headache with conjunctival injection and tearing. Ann Neurol. 1999;46(5):791–4. 64. McMahon SB. Wall and Melzack’s textbook of pain. 6th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier/ Saunders; 2013. xxix, 1153 p. 65. Melzack R, Casey KL. In: Kenshalo D, editor.. The skin senses Sensory, motivational, and central control determinants of pain: a new conceptual model. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas; 1968. p. 423–39. 66. Merskey H, Bogduk N, International Association for the Study of Pain. Classification of chronic pain: descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Seattle: IASP Press; 1994. xvi, 222 p. 67. Moulton EA, Burstein R, Tully S, Hargreaves R, Becerra L, Borsook D. Interictal dysfunction of a brainstem descending modulatory center in migraine patients. PLoS One. 2008;3(11):e3799. 68. Nascimento TD, DosSantos MF, Lucas S, van Holsbeeck H, DeBoer M, Maslowski E, et al. μ-Opioid activation in the midbrain during migraine allodynia - brief report II. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2014;1(6):445–50. 69. Niddam DM, Lai KL, Fuh JL, Chuang CY, Chen WT, Wang SJ. Reduced functional connectivity between salience and visual networks in migraine with aura. Cephalalgia. 2016;36(1):53–66. 70. Ossipov MH, Morimura K, Porreca F. Descending pain modulation and chronification of pain. Curr Opin Support Palliat Care. 2014;8(2):143–51. 71. Qiu E, Wang Y, Ma L, Tian L, Liu R, Dong Z, et al. Abnormal brain functional connectivity of the hypothalamus in cluster headaches. PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e57896. 72. Sarchielli P, Tarducci R, Presciutti O, Gobbi G, Pelliccioli GP, Stipa G, et al. Functional 1H-MRS findings in migraine patients with and without aura assessed interictally. NeuroImage. 2005;24(4):1025–31. 73. Schrepf A, Harper DE, Harte SE, Wang H, Ichesco E, Hampson JP, et al. Endogenous opioidergic dysregulation of pain in fibromyalgia: a PET and fMRI study. Pain. 2016;157(10):2217–25. 74. Schulte LH, May A. Functional neuroimaging in migraine: chances and challenges. Headache. 2016;56(9):1474–81. 75. Schulte LH, May A. The migraine generator revisited: continuous scanning of the migraine cycle over 30 days and three spontaneous attacks. Brain. 2016;139.(Pt 7:1987–93. 76. Schulte LH, Allers A, May A. Hypothalamus as a mediator of chronic migraine: evidence from high-resolution fMRI. Neurology. 2017;88(21):2011–6. 77. Schwenkreis P, Scherens A, Rönnau AK, Höffken O, Tegenthoff M, Maier C. Cortical disinhibition occurs in chronic neuropathic, but not in chronic nociceptive pain. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11:73. 78. Sprenger T, Boecker H, Tolle TR, Bussone G, May A, Leone M. Specific hypothalamic activation during a spontaneous cluster headache attack. Neurology. 2004;62(3):516–7. 79. Sprenger T, Valet M, Platzer S, Pfaffenrath V, Steude U, Tolle TR. SUNCT: bilateral hypothalamic activation during headache attacks and resolving of symptoms after trigeminal decompression. Pain. 2005;113(3):422–6. 80. Stankewitz A, May A. Increased limbic and brainstem activity during migraine attacks following olfactory stimulation. Neurology. 2011;77(5):476–82. 81. Stankewitz A, Aderjan D, Eippert F, May A. Trigeminal nociceptive transmission in migraineurs predicts migraine attacks. J Neurosci. 2011;31(6):1937–43. 82. Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Katsarava Z, Lainez JM, Lampl C, Lantéri-Minet M, et al. The impact of headache in Europe: principal results of the Eurolight project. J Headache Pain. 2014;15:31. 83. Steiner TJ, Birbeck GL, Jensen RH, Katsarava Z, Stovner LJ, Martelletti P. Headache disorders are third cause of disability worldwide. J Headache Pain. 2015;16:58. 3 Mechanisms of Pain and Headache 41 84. Stovner L, Hagen K, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Lipton R, Scher A, et al. The global burden of headache: a documentation of headache prevalence and disability worldwide. Cephalalgia. 2007;27(3):193–210. 85. Tedeschi G, Russo A, Conte F, Corbo D, Caiazzo G, Giordano A, et al. Increased interictal visual network connectivity in patients with migraine with aura. Cephalalgia. 2016;36(2):139–47. 86. Vachon-Presseau E, Tétreault P, Petre B, Huang L, Berger SE, Torbey S, et al. Corticolimbic anatomical characteristics predetermine risk for chronic pain. Brain. 2016;139(Pt 7):1958–70. 87. Verhaak PF, Kerssens JJ, Dekker J, Sorbi MJ, Bensing JM. Prevalence of chronic benign pain disorder among adults: a review of the literature. Pain. 1998;77(3):231–9. 88. Weiller C, May A, Limmroth V, Jüptner M, Kaube H, Schayck RV, et al. Brain stem activation in spontaneous human migraine attacks. Nat Med. 1995;1(7):658–60. 89. Willoch F, Schindler F, Wester HJ, Empl M, Straube A, Schwaiger M, et al. Central poststroke pain and reduced opioid receptor binding within pain processing circuitries: a [11C]diprenorphine PET study. Pain. 2004;108(3):213–20. 90. Wood PB, Schweinhardt P, Jaeger E, Dagher A, Hakyemez H, Rabiner EA, et al. Fibromyalgia patients show an abnormal dopamine response to pain. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25(12):3576–82. 91. Xue T, Yuan K, Zhao L, Yu D, Dong T, Cheng P, et al. Intrinsic brain network abnormalities in migraines without aura revealed in resting-state fMRI. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e52927. 92. Yang FC, Chou KH, Fuh JL, Lee PL, Lirng JF, Lin YY, et al. Altered hypothalamic functional connectivity in cluster headache: a longitudinal resting-state functional MRI study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2015;86(4):437–45. 93. Zhang J, Su J, Wang M, Zhao Y, Yao Q, Zhang Q, et al. Increased default mode network connectivity and increased regional homogeneity in migraineurs without aura. J Headache Pain. 2016;17(1):98. 94. Zubieta JK. Pain signal as threat and reward. Neuron. 2010;66(1):6–7. 95. Zubieta J, Smith Y, Bueller J, Xu Y, Kilbourn M, Jewett D, et al. Regional mu opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. Science. 2001;293(5528):311–5. Chapter 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo Elisa Carlino, Lene Vase, and Alessandro Piedimonte 4.1 Introduction A placebo agent is usually defined as a substance, device, or procedure without active properties, whereas the placebo effect is defined as the positive response following the administration of a placebo [10, 27, 39, 83]. Historically, placebos are used in medical settings to please patients or in clinical trials to control the effectiveness of an active treatment [18, 47, 50, 97]. Today, however, it is evident that the conceptualization of placebo is more complex as placebo and nocebo effects are influenced by different external, internal, and relational elements representing the “atmosphere around the treatment” [8, 24]. The external elements refer to the physical context surrounding the medical treatment and include the physical properties of the treatment itself, e.g., its color, shape, taste, and smell, as well as the elements characterizing the place where the therapy is administered, e.g., the presence of medical staff and of different medical instruments. The internal elements are represented by the patient’s characteristics, such as personal beliefs, expectations, and emotions in relation to the therapeutic outcome, memories about previous medical treatments, as well as different psychological traits and genetic variables. The relational elements are represented by all the social cues characterizing the patient-doctor relationship, such as the verbal information that the doctor gives to the patient, the communication style, or the body language [28, 86, 93, 100, 105]. When a treatment is delivered, the patient’s clinical improvement can be influenced by several factors such as spontaneous remission, regression to the mean, E. Carlino (*) · A. Piedimonte Department of Neuroscience, University of Turin Medical School, Turin, Italy e-mail: [email protected] L. Vase Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, School of Business and Social Sciences, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_4 43 44 E. Carlino et al. patient and doctor biases, and unidentified effects of co-interventions [11]. Thus, to isolate the genuine effect of the context, it is crucial to control for these confounding factors, for example, by including no-treatment control conditions. There is not one single placebo effect or nocebo effect but many. Placebo and nocebo effects involve different mechanisms across various diseases and therapeutic interventions [10]. Still, the majority of our knowledge of the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms involved in placebo and nocebo effects comes from the field of pain. In this chapter, we will therefore focus on pain, a widespread phenomenon with a huge clinical impact. First, we outline the psychological mechanisms underlying placebo and nocebo effects, including expectations, learning, and their interaction. Second, we describe the neurobiological, neuroanatomical, and neurochemical underpinnings of these phenomena. Finally, we discuss ethical and conscious use of placebo and nocebo factors in clinical practice. 4.2 Psychological Mechanisms From a psychological point of view, two main mechanisms have been proposed to explain both placebo and nocebo effects, namely, cognitive expectation and learning processes. The first mechanism, expectation, can be defined as the experienced likelihood of an outcome [83], and it represents an evolutionary advantage since it prepares the organism to cope with the expected positive or negative effects related to a future event [19]. This mechanism can lead to placebo analgesia or nocebo hyperalgesia through the subjective belief of receiving an effective treatment that can, respectively, reduce [1, 17, 103] or increase [12, 35] pain perception. During a clinical treatment, this belief is often induced by the clinician’s verbal suggestions [57, 83] and/or by the experience of a warm and empathic relationship. For instance, different studies on healthy participants have shown that applying a placebo analgesic cream on three contiguous skin areas produced different levels of analgesia, depending on which verbal suggestion was provided: strong effects were found when the cream was defined as a strong analgesic, and weak effects were found if the cream was defined as a weak analgesic [83]. The same phenomenon was observed in treatments for irritable bowel syndrome as well as in postoperative settings. Indeed, patients with irritable bowel syndrome who received an “augmented” placebo acupuncture treatment, where practitioners created a warmer and more attentive relationship, resulted in a higher symptoms’ reduction and an overall stronger improvement of the quality of life in comparison to patients who received a standard placebo acupuncture [54]. Finally, in postoperative settings, patients receiving a physiological (i.e., placebo) infusion showed a lower demand of additional painkillers when the infusion was defined as a powerful painkiller, while this demand became higher when the infusion was correctly described as a placebo [79]. Interestingly, expectations about a treatment are not only generated by verbal 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo 45 s­ uggestions; rather, the participant/patient gathers information from all contextual cues accompanying a treatment. For instance, branded analgesic tablets are more effective in relieving headaches than their unbranded counterparts just as subcutaneous placebo administration is more powerful than oral administration in the treatment of migraine [7, 25, 43]. Furthermore, other cognitive factors influence expectation [48, 83]: self-efficacy, i.e., the belief of being capable of managing adverse events and inducing positive changes, and self-reinforcing or “somatic focus,” i.e., the process of taking any improvement sign in a therapy as a crucial evidence of successful treatment, disregarding all opposite evidence. All these cognitive factors modulate subjective expectations about a specific treatment and, in turn, increase or decrease the outcome of a medical treatment. In particular, expectations may lead to these effects in combination with desire for pain relief and reduction in negative emotions like anxiety, possibly by tapping into rewards systems. For instance, administering a placebo treatment to patients affected by irritable bowel syndrome has been shown to reduce their anxiety level, and this reduction was significantly correlated to their pain relief [98]. Furthermore, within a clinical model of visceral pain, it has been shown that participants who are more influenced by negative expectations induced by verbal suggestions show an increased functional coupling between the insula and midcingulate cortex that leads to an exacerbation of pain induced by repetitive rectal stimulations [88]. Aside from cognitive expectations, learning mechanisms are crucial to placebo and nocebo effects. Patients have a medical history, and during their lifetime they gain experience with how different treatments produce specific clinical outcomes. This knowledge is acquired through a process of classical conditioning where, for instance, an analgesic substance, the so-called unconditioned stimulus, is associated with a specific ritual of administration, “the conditioned stimulus,” leading to an analgesic response, that is, the “unconditioned response.” After repeated associations, the simple rehearsal of the administration ritual can lead the patient to experience the analgesic response, now defined as a “conditioned response” [39, 84, 106]. Placebo analgesia induced via learning processes is typically stronger and tends to last longer in comparison to analgesic effects generated by verbal suggestions alone. This difference has been observed in several experiments including behavioral [29, 34], neuroimaging [69], and electrophysiological studies [72, 77] as well as in clinical settings [58]. Conditioned placebo analgesia is affected by efficacy of the treatment exposure as well as the number of learning sessions: in other words, the greater the benefit from previous treatment and the higher the number of learning sessions, the stronger and longer-lasting the conditioned analgesia [34, 40, 72, 77]. This learning effect is less robust when conditioned nocebo hyperalgesia is induced [60]. Indeed nocebo effects seem less related to learning phenomena [41] and more resistant to extinction [33]. Whereas cognitive expectations are to a high extent mediated by conscious processes, classical conditioning seems to relate to unconscious physiological processes [59]. For instance, healthy participants who repeatedly received a flavored drink as conditioned stimulus associated with the immunosuppressive drug cyclosporine A showed a conditioned immunosuppression, even after the administration 46 E. Carlino et al. of the drink alone, as assessed by lymphocyte, interleukin-2, and interferon-gamma proliferation [49]. Even though learning via classical conditioning has been described simply as the unconscious association between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, more recent cognitive theories regarding this mechanism state that the information contained in the conditioned stimulus, e.g., a colored pill, leads to a specific conscious expectation that a given event, e.g., the analgesic effect, will follow the conditioned stimulus administration [71, 85, 99]. Thus, these mechanisms do not work independently of one another; rather, their combination results in different placebo and nocebo effects, and their interaction may enhance or hamper these effects [26, 58, 59]. Indeed, it has been shown in healthy participants that verbally induced expectations of analgesia or hyperalgesia contrasted and eliminated opposite effects elicited by conditioning procedures [21]. Recently, significant placebo and nocebo effects on pain were also induced using subliminal stimuli, indicating that, at least in some circumstances, placebo and nocebo effects can operate without conscious awareness [53, 60]. 4.3 Neuroanatomical and Neurochemical Underpinnings The cascade of neurobiological changes triggered by the psychosocial context has been studied extensively [10, 19, 24, 27, 37, 44, 48, 101]. Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) have identified different brain regions and brain pathways that contribute to placebo and nocebo responses (see Fig. 4.1a). These regions include a complex cortical-brainstem system involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), lateral orbitofrontal cortex (LOFC), nucleus accumbens (NAc), periaqueductal gray (PAG), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and rostroventral medulla (RVM) [51, 61, 68, 70, 75, 76, 82, 89, 90, 95, 101, 103, 104, 107, 108]. In particular, during the anticipation phase, when an analgesic effect is expected, activation of ACC, precentral and lateral prefrontal cortex, and PAG has been documented. During pain inhibition, i.e., during the analgesic phase, a deactivation has been found in different brain regions such as the mid- and posterior cingulate cortex, superior temporal and precentral gyri, the anterior and posterior insula, the claustrum and putamen, and the thalamus and caudate body [73]. On the contrary, when pain exacerbation is expected, a subjective increase in pain rating has been reported along with an increased activity in different brain regions involved in pain processing and emotion regulation, such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC), ACC, and insula [32, 55, 60, 62, 63, 78, 80, 81, 87]. Recently, connectivity studies have shown that during placebo analgesia coupling between brain regions, including those involved in cognitive processes (e.g., attention, expectation, evaluation), was significantly enhanced (see Fig. 4.1b). Specifically, a significantly consistent decrease in the coupling between DLPFC and PAG was found [91, 92]. Furthermore, the extent of right midfrontal gyrus connectivity predicts placebo responses across chronic pain clinical trials [94]. Brain 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo a 47 c Morphine Placebo Naloxone Anticipation of pain Inhibition of pain Exacerbation of pain relief Prefrontal cortex MCC and PCC Prefrontal cortex ACC Superior temporal and precentral gyri PAG ACC insula µ-opioid receptors ANALGESIA Thalamus Insula and claustrum Anticipatory anxiety µ-opioid receptors µ-opioid receptors Placebo ANALGESIA Block of Placebo ANALGESIA Proglumide Putamen and caudate body CCK receptors b Increase of Nocebo HYPERALGESIA Placebo Analgesia Nocebo Hyperanalgesia DLPFC Operculum PAG Basal Ganglia Ketorolac Pentagastrin CCK CCK-2 receptors receptors Increase of Placebo Block of ANALGESIA Placebo ANALGESIA Placebo Rimonabant Naloxone CB1 receptors CB1 receptors CB1 receptors ANALGESIA Placebo ANALGESIA Block of Placebo ANALGESIA Fig. 4.1 Principal neurobiological mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects. (a) Neuroanatomy of placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia have been described through different brain imaging studies. (b) Connectivity studies have shown that coupling between different brain regions occurs during placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. (c) The opioid and cannabinoid systems are involved in placebo and nocebo effects. In some circumstances, placebo analgesia occurs through the activation of the opioid system and can be reversed by naloxone. Anticipatory anxiety can activate the pro-nociceptive cholecystokinin (CCK) system, leading to nocebo hyperalgesia. The pro-nociceptive CCK effect can be reversed by proglumide and agonized by pentagastrin. Placebos can also activate the CB1 cannabinoid receptors, producing an analgesic effect that can be reversed by rimonabant c­onnectivity seems to be affected also by negative verbal suggestions; indeed, nocebo hyperalgesia seems to be accompanied by the activation of the operculum over an extended time period, and the operculum exhibited changes in coupling over time during nociceptive input, as demonstrated by decreased connectivity with the basal ganglia [46]. Pharmacological studies on placebo analgesia have so far documented the activation of two different neurochemical systems: the opioid and cannabinoid systems (see Fig. 4.1c). The opioid system is the most studied and understood. The observation that μ-opioid antagonist naloxone blocks some types of placebo analgesia has been documented using conditioning protocols, indicating that the opioid system plays a pivotal role. In particular, it has been shown that when opioid drugs like morphine are administered for several consecutive days and subsequently replaced by a placebo, the ensuing placebo effect can be reversed by naloxone [1, 45, 65]. Further confirmation of the involvement of the opioid system comes from the study of the anti-opioid action of cholecystokinin (CCK); indeed, the CCK antagonist 48 E. Carlino et al. proglumide enhances placebo analgesia [9, 13], whereas pentagastrin, a CCK-2 receptor agonist, disrupts it [15]. CCK also mediates nocebo hyperalgesic responses; for example, nocebo hyperalgesia induced by expectations of pain and anxiety increase can be reversed by the CCK antagonist proglumide [12, 16]. The involvement of the cannabinoid system has recently been documented using conditioning protocols with non-opioid drugs such as ketorolac. After a pharmacological preconditioning with ketorolac, the placebo analgesic response cannot be reversed by naloxone but by the CB1 cannabinoid antagonist rimonabant [14]. The involvement of this system has also been confirmed by the study of the functional missense variant Pro129Thr of the gene coding for fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH), the major degrading enzyme of endocannabinoids [74]. Also, electroencephalographic (EEG) studies have revealed insights into the dynamical and temporal changes that occur before and after the administration of a placebo treatment. Indeed, as suggested by Sevel et al. [91], small placebo effects could be better described by changes in the temporal dynamics in pain modulatory regions than by changes in the magnitude of blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation, and EEG is a good tool for investigating temporal changes in neural activity. Different studies have demonstrated that placebo treatments produce a reduction of pain along with a significant decrease in pain-evoked event-related potentials (ERPs), suggesting that placebo analgesia affects early nociceptive processes [30, 42, 102] but also expectation of upcoming nociceptive stimuli [77]. Finally, studies on frequency analysis on ongoing (resting) EEG have proved that alpha activity, known to be influenced by top-down processes, increased significantly after a placebo treatment [52]. Moreover, the subjectively reported decrease of pain intensity correlates with the increase in the amplitude of alpha oscillations during pain conditions in frontal-central regions [67]. 4.4 Clinical Implications The demonstration that the context surrounding a medical treatment can influence its effectiveness has crucial clinical implications. First, the presence of the context and the relationship with a healthcare provider are pivotal in order to maximize the effectiveness of a medical treatment, as extensively demonstrated by the comparison of “open” versus “hidden” interventions. A classic example of an “open” intervention is the administration of medical treatment in full view of the patient (e.g., with a doctor at the bedside who explains and performs the procedure to the patient, describing its therapeutic effects). On the contrary, the “hidden” intervention is the administration of a treatment unbeknownst to the patient and without any previous advice from a doctor or nurse (e.g., using a machine performing an automatic infusion). If a treatment is administered without any explicit information, the patient cannot be aware of the treatment itself, and, in turn, this lack of awareness does not elicit positive expectations and/or reductions in negative emotions, thus leading to reduced treatment efficacy [20, 38]. For example, in seminal studies on placebo 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo 49 analgesia, it has been observed that an open injection of saline solution, believed by the patient to have analgesic properties, produced the same analgesic effects as a hidden injection of 6–8 mg of morphine [64, 66]. The differential efficacy of open and hidden interventions has been confirmed for different painkillers, such as buprenorphine, tramadol, ketorolac, and metamizole [3, 21, 38]. Brain areas linked to open or hidden administration of analgesic drugs have been identified by means of neuroimaging techniques. An fMRI study showed that, compared to hidden administration, open administration of the opioid agonist remifentanil significantly increased analgesia and that this increase correlated with higher activity in DLPFC and pregenual ACC. In addition, when participants were deceivingly informed that the administration of remifentanil would be interrupted, the analgesic effect was completely abolished even in the presence of the drug, and this analgesia loss was associated with increased activity in the hippocampus [23]. Furthermore, using an open-hidden design to manipulate the participants’ knowledge of drug administration, it has been observed that brain areas activated by positive expectation of receiving an analgesic drug, e.g., remifentanil, differ from areas activated by its sole pharmacodynamics action [6]. Indeed, while both remifentanil and expectancy reduce pain, positive expectation modulates activity in the frontal cortex with a separate time course from drug effects, thus showing that expectations and drugs can operate without mutual interference. A second clinical implication is that the assessment of a patient’s medical history is essential to treatment optimization. For example, when healthy participants were exposed to a negative treatment (negative history group), the placebo analgesic response to a second treatment was lower compared to participants exposed to a positive treatment (positive history group) [56]. Also, the decrease of analgesia in participants who first experienced an unsuccessful treatment correlated with higher brain activity in areas involved in pain processing, e.g., the insular cortex, as well as with lower brain activity in areas involved in pain inhibition and placebo analgesia, e.g., DLPFC [56]. Moreover, changing of the route of drug administration (e.g., from topical to oral) doesn’t counteract a negative treatment history, indicating that learned carry-over effects generalize over time and across routes of drug administration [109]. Following a similar approach in patients with chronic pain treated with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), it has been observed that successful active stimulation of the motor cortex followed by sessions of sham rTMS produced 11% of pain reduction, while unsuccessful sessions of rTMS followed by sham rTMS resulted in an opposite pain increase by 6% [4]. It is interesting to notice that these patients were all resistant to pharmacological treatments, but placebo interventions still held positive results after effective sessions of rTMS, highlighting how conditioned analgesic responses can still be elicited and enhanced in clinical treatments in these patients [37]. Third, in the clinical setting, it is crucial to avoid nocebo effects [22, 31, 36, 93]. Even though the study of the nocebo effect has received less attention, mainly due to ethical constraints, it is well documented that a negative context can generate negative therapeutic outcomes. For example, negative information provided during the administration of an analgesic treatment can reverse the topical analgesic effect 50 E. Carlino et al. of the treatment itself [5] or exacerbate pain during invasive procedures [96]. Communication of a negative diagnosis or side effects is also a potentially harmful situation in the clinical setting. For example, comparing the rates of adverse events reported in the placebo arms of clinical trials for three classes of anti-migraine drugs, the rate of adverse events in the placebo arms was high. Moreover, the adverse events in the placebo arms corresponded to those of the anti-migraine medication with which the placebo was compared [2]. All the abovementioned studies emphasize that first, the information delivered by healthcare professionals as well as the environment where the treatment is administered is crucial for the outcome of a specific therapy, since less information could translate into diminished efficacy. Second, the specific clinical history of each patient is important to better understand how he/she will react to a new treatment and to construct a clinical setting tailored to his/her expectations. Third, a negative therapeutic context can compromise the effectiveness of a medical treatment so preventing nocebo effects is of major importance in the clinical setting. 4.5 Conclusions Current knowledge of placebo and nocebo effects provides evidence of the key role of the psychosocial context accompanying the administration of medical treatments. Different psychological mechanisms have been recognized as mediators of placebo and nocebo effects, especially the relationship with the healthcare provider, expectation, emotions, and learning processes. Moreover, neuroimaging and psychopharmacological studies support the placebo and nocebo research, documenting the involvement of specific neurochemical systems and the activation/deactivation of different brain regions. More research is needed to increase our understanding of these phenomena in order to apply these findings in the clinical setting with the aim of improving personalized approaches and increase the effectiveness of medical treatments. References 1. Amanzio M, Benedetti F. Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J Neurosci. 1999;19:484–94. 2. Amanzio M, Corazzini LL, Vase L, Benedetti F. A systematic review of adverse events in placebo groups of anti-migraine clinical trials. Pain. 2009;146:261–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. pain.2009.07.010. 3. Amanzio M, Pollo A, Maggi G, Benedetti F. Response variability to analgesics: a role for nonspecific activation of endogenous opioids. Pain. 2001;90:205–15. 4. André-Obadia N, Magnin M, Garcia-Larrea L. On the importance of placebo timing in rTMS studies for pain relief. Pain. 2011;152:1233–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.027. 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo 51 5. Aslaksen PM, Zwarg ML, Eilertsen H-IH, Gorecka MM, Bjørkedal E. Opposite effects of the same drug: reversal of topical analgesia by nocebo information. Pain. 2015;156:39–46. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000004. 6. Atlas LY, Whittington RA, Lindquist MA, Wielgosz J, Sonty N, Wager TD. Dissociable influences of opiates and expectations on pain. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8053–64. https://doi. org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0383-12.2012. 7. Autret A, Valade D, Debiais S. Placebo and other psychological interactions in headache treatment. J Headache Pain. 2012;13:191–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10194-012-0422-0. 8. BALINT M. The doctor, his patient, and the illness. Lancet. 1955;268:683–8. 9. Benedetti F. The opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia. Pain. 1996;64:535–43. 10. Benedetti F. Placebo effects: from the neurobiological paradigm to translational implications. Neuron. 2014;84:623–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.023. 11. Benedetti F. Placebo effects. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2014. 12. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Casadio C, Oliaro A, Maggi G. Blockade of nocebo hyperalgesia by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide. Pain. 1997;71:135–40. 13. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Maggi G. Potentiation of placebo analgesia by proglumide. Lancet. 1995;346:1231. 14. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Rosato R, Blanchard C. Nonopioid placebo analgesia is mediated by CB1 cannabinoid receptors. Nat Med. 2011;17:1228–30. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.2435. 15. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Thoen W. Disruption of opioid-induced placebo responses by activation of cholecystokinin type-2 receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;213:791–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2037-y. 16. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Vighetti S, Asteggiano G. The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J Neurosci. 2006;26:12014–22. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.2947-06.2006. 17. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M. Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by targetdirected expectations of analgesia. J Neurosci. 1999;19:3639–48. 18. Benedetti F, Carlino E, Piedimonte A. Increasing uncertainty in CNS clinical trials: the role of placebo, nocebo, and Hawthorne effects. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15:736–47. https://doi. org/10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00066-1. 19. Benedetti F, Carlino E, Pollo A. How placebos change the patient’s brain. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:339–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.81. 20. Benedetti F, Carlino E, Pollo A. Hidden administration of drugs. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2011;90:651–61. https://doi.org/10.1038/clpt.2011.206. 21. Benedetti F, Maggi G, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Rainero I, Vighetti S, Pollo A. Open versus hidden medical treatments: the patient’s knowledge about a therapy affects the therapy outcome. Prev Treat. 2003;6. https://doi.org/10.1037/1522-3736.6.1.61a. 22. Bingel U. Avoiding nocebo effects to optimize treatment outcome. JAMA. 2014;312:693–4. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2014.8342. 23. Bingel U, Wanigasekera V, Wiech K, Ni Mhuircheartaigh R, Lee MC, Ploner M, Tracey I. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci Transl Med. 2011;3:70ra14. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed. 3001244. 24. Di BZ, Harkness E, Ernst E, Georgiou A, Kleijnen J. Influence of context effects on health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2001;357:757–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0140-6736(00)04169-6. 25. Branthwaite A, Cooper P. Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1981;282:1576–8. 26. Büchel C, Geuter S, Sprenger C, Eippert F. Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective. Neuron. 2014;81:1223–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042. 27. Carlino E, Benedetti F. Placebo and nocebo effects. In: Mostofsky DI, editor. The handbook of behavioral medicine. 2014. p. 36–57. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118453940.ch3. 52 E. Carlino et al. 28. Carlino E, Benedetti F. Different contexts, different pains, different experiences. Neuroscience. 2016;338:19–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.053. 29. Carlino E, Guerra G, Piedimonte A. Placebo effects: from pain to motor performance. Neurosci Lett. 2016;632:224–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2016.08.046. 30. Carlino E, Torta D, Piedimonte A, Frisaldi E, Vighetti S, Benedetti F. Role of explicit verbal information in conditioned analgesia. Eur J Pain. 2015;19:546–53. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ejp.579. 31. Chavarria V, Vian J, Pereira C, Data-Franco J, Fernandes BS, Berk M, Dodd S. The placebo and nocebo phenomena: their clinical management and impact on treatment outcomes. Clin Ther. 2017;39:477–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinthera.2017.01.031. 32. Chua P, Krams M, Toni I, Passingham R, Dolan R. A functional anatomy of anticipatory anxiety. Neuroimage. 1999;9:563–71. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0407. 33. Colagiuri B, Quinn VF. Autonomic arousal as a mechanism of the persistence of nocebo hyperalgesia. J Pain. 2018;19:476–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2017.12.006. 34. Colloca L, Benedetti F. How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia. Pain. 2006;124:126– 33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005. 35. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2007;20:435–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb. 36. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA. 2012;307:567–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.115. 37. Colloca L, Grillon C. Understanding placebo and nocebo responses for pain management. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2014;18:419. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11916-014-0419-2. 38. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:679–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S1474-4422(04)00908-1. 39. Colloca L, Miller FG. How placebo responses are formed: a learning perspective. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1859–69. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0398. 40. Colloca L, Petrovic P, Wager TD, Ingvar M, Benedetti F. How the number of learning trials affects placebo and nocebo responses. Pain. 2010;151:430–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. pain.2010.08.007. 41. Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. Pain. 2008;136:211–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006. 42. Colloca L, Tinazzi M, Recchia S, Le Pera D, Fiaschi A, Benedetti F, Valeriani M. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. Pain. 2008;139:306–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.021. 43. de Craen AJ, Tijssen JG, de Gans J, Kleijnen J. Placebo effect in the acute treatment of migraine: subcutaneous placebos are better than oral placebos. J Neurol. 2000;247:183–8. 44. Dodd S, Dean OM, Vian J, Berk M. A review of the theoretical and biological understanding of the nocebo and placebo phenomena. Clin Ther. 2017;39:469–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. clinthera.2017.01.010. 45. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63:533–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. 46. Ellerbrock I, Wiehler A, Arndt M, May A. Nocebo context modulates long-term habituation to heat pain and influences functional connectivity of the operculum. Pain. 2015;156:2222–33. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000297. 47. Enck P, Klosterhalfen S, Weimer K, Horing B, Zipfel S. The placebo response in clinical trials: more questions than answers. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1889–95. https://doi. org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0384. 48. Frisaldi E, Piedimonte A, Benedetti F. Placebo and nocebo effects: a complex interplay between psychological factors and neurochemical networks. Am J Clin Hypn. 2015;57:267–84. https:// doi.org/10.1080/00029157.2014.976785. 49. Goebel MU, Trebst AE, Steiner J, Xie YF, Exton MS, Frede S, Canbay AE, Michel MC, Heemann U, Schedlowski M. Behavioral conditioning of immunosuppression is possible in humans. FASEB J. 2002;16:1869–73. https://doi.org/10.1096/fj.02-0389com. 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo 53 50. Gupta U, Verma M. Placebo in clinical trials. Perspect Clin Res. 2013;4:49–52. https://doi. org/10.4103/2229-3485.106383. 51. Hashmi JA, Baria AT, Baliki MN, Huang L, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV. Brain networks predicting placebo analgesia in a clinical trial for chronic back pain. Pain. 2012;153:2393–402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.008. 52. Huneke NTM, Brown CA, Burford E, Watson A, Trujillo-Barreto NJ, El-Deredy W, AKP J. Experimental placebo analgesia changes resting-state alpha oscillations. PLoS One. 2013;8:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078278. 53. Jensen KB, Kaptchuk TJ, Kirsch I, Raicek J, Lindstrom KM, Berna C, Gollub RL, Ingvar M, Kong J. Nonconscious activation of placebo and nocebo pain responses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:15959–64. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1202056109. 54. Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, Davis RB, Kerr CE, Jacobson EE, Kirsch I, Schyner RN, Nam BH, Nguyen LT, Park M, Rivers AL, McManus C, Kokkotou E, Drossman DA, Goldman P, Lembo AJ. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336:999–1003. https://doi.org/10.1136/ bmj.39524.439618.25. 55. Keltner JR, Furst A, Fan C, Redfern R, Inglis B, Fields HL. Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4437–43. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4463-05.2006. 56. Kessner S, Wiech K, Forkmann K, Ploner M, Bingel U. The effect of treatment history on therapeutic outcome: an experimental approach. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173:1468–9. https:// doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.6705. 57. Kirsch I, Wickless C, Moffitt KH. Expectancy and suggestibility: are the effects of environmental enhancement due to detection? Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1999;47:40–5. https://doi. org/10.1080/00207149908410021. 58. Klinger R, Soost S, Flor H, Worm M. Classical conditioning and expectancy in placebo hypoalgesia: a randomized controlled study in patients with atopic dermatitis and persons with healthy skin. Pain. 2007;128:31–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.025. 59. Kong J, Benedetti F. Placebo and nocebo effects: an introduction to psychological and biological mechanisms. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2014;225:3–15. https://doi. org/10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_1. 60. Kong J, Gollub RL, Polich G, Kirsch I, Laviolette P, Vangel M, Rosen B, Kaptchuk TJ. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on the neural mechanisms of hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13354–62. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-08.2008. 61. Kong J, Gollub RL, Rosman IS, Webb JM, Vangel MG, Kirsch I, Kaptchuk TJ. Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2006;26:381–8. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.3556-05.2006. 62. Koyama T, McHaffie JG, Laurienti PJ, Coghill RC. The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:12950–5. https://doi. org/10.1073/pnas.0408576102. 63. Koyama T, Tanaka YZ, Mikami A. Nociceptive neurons in the macaque anterior cingulate activate during anticipation of pain. Neuroreport. 1998;9:2663–7. 64. Levine JD, Gordon NC. Influence of the method of drug administration on analgesic response. Nature. 1984;312:755–6. 65. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL. The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet. 1978;2:654–7. 66. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Smith R, Fields HL. Analgesic responses to morphine and placebo in individuals with postoperative pain. Pain. 1981;10:379–89. 67. Li L, Wang H, Ke X, Liu X, Yuan Y, Zhang D, Xiong D, Qiu Y. Placebo analgesia changes alpha oscillations induced by tonic muscle pain: EEG frequency analysis including data during pain evaluation. Front Comput Neurosci. 2016;10:45. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2016. 00045. 68. Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM, Berman S, Naliboff BD, Suyenobu BY, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA. The neural correlates of placebo effects: a disruption account. Neuroimage. 2004;22:447– 55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.037. 54 E. Carlino et al. 69. Lui F, Colloca L, Duzzi D, Anchisi D, Benedetti F, Porro CA. Neural bases of conditioned placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;151:816–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.021. 70. Meissner K, Bingel U, Colloca L, Wager TD, Watson A, Flaten MA. The placebo effect: advances from different methodological approaches. J Neurosci. 2011;31:16117–24. https:// doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4099-11.2011. 71. Montgomery G, Kirsch I. Mechanisms of placebo pain reduction: an empirical investigation. Psychol Sci. 1996;7:174–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00352.x. 72. Morton DL, C a B, Watson A, El-Deredy W, Jones AKP. Cognitive changes as a result of a single exposure to placebo. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1958–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuropsychologia.2010.03.016. 73. Palermo S, Benedetti F, Costa T, Amanzio M. Pain anticipation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:1648–61. https:// doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22727. 74. Peciña M, Martínez-Jauand M, Hodgkinson C, Stohler CS, Goldman D, Zubieta JK. FAAH selectively influences placebo effects. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:385–91. https://doi. org/10.1038/mp.2013.124. 75. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Andersson J, Fransson P, Ingvar M. A prefrontal nonopioid mechanism in placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;150:59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. pain.2010.03.011. 76. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia—imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–40. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1067176. 77. Piedimonte A, Guerra G, Vighetti S, Carlino E. Measuring expectation of pain: contingent negative variation in placebo and nocebo effects. Eur J Pain. 2017;21:874–85. https://doi. org/10.1002/ejp.990. 78. Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science. 1999;284:1979–81. 79. Pollo A, Amanzio M, Arslanian A, Casadio C, Maggi G, Benedetti F. Response expectancies in placebo analgesia and their clinical relevance. Pain. 2001;93:77–84. 80. Porro CA, Baraldi P, Pagnoni G, Serafini M, Facchin P, Maieron M, Nichelli P. Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? J Neurosci. 2002;22:3206–14. doi: 20026310 81. Porro CA, Cettolo V, Francescato MP, Baraldi P. Functional activity mapping of the mesial hemispheric wall during anticipation of pain. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1738–47. 82. Price DD, Craggs J, Verne GN, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME. Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients. Pain. 2007;127:63–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.08.001. 83. Price DD, Finniss DG, Benedetti F. A comprehensive review of the placebo effect: recent advances and current thought. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:565–90. https://doi.org/10.1146/ annurev.psych.59.113006.095941. 84. Price DD, Milling LS, Kirsch I, Duff A, Montgomery GH, Nicholls SS. An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. Pain. 1999;83:147–56. 85. Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning: it’s not what you think it is. Am Psychol. 1988;43(3):151–60. 86. Rossetti G, Carlino E, Testa M. Clinical relevance of contextual factors as triggers of placebo and nocebo effects in musculoskeletal pain. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2017;19:27. 87. Sawamoto N, Honda M, Okada T, Hanakawa T, Kanda M, Fukuyama H, Konishi J, Shibasaki H. Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2000;20:7438–45. 88. Schmid J, Bingel U, Ritter C, Benson S, Schedlowski M, Gramsch C, Forsting M, Elsenbruch S. Neural underpinnings of nocebo hyperalgesia in visceral pain: a fMRI study in healthy volunteers. Neuroimage. 2015;120:114–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.060. 4 Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo 55 89. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta J-K. Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron. 2007;55:325–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.028. 90. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta J-K. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:220–31. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34. 91. Sevel LS, Craggs JG, Price DD, Staud R, Robinson ME. Placebo analgesia enhances descending pain-related effective connectivity: a dynamic causal modeling study of endogenous pain modulation. J Pain. 2015;16:760–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.001. 92. Sevel LS, O’Shea AM, Letzen JE, Craggs JG, Price DD, Robinson ME. Effective connectivity predicts future placebo analgesic response: a dynamic causal modeling study of pain processing in healthy controls. Neuroimage. 2015;110:87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2015.01.056. 93. Testa M, Rossettini G. Enhance placebo, avoid nocebo: how contextual factors affect physiotherapy outcomes. Man Ther. 2016;24:65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2016.04.006. 94. Tétreault P, Mansour A, Vachon-Presseau E, Schnitzer TJ, Apkarian AV, Baliki MN. Brain connectivity predicts placebo response across chronic pain clinical trials. PLoS Biol. 2016;14:e1002570. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002570. 95. Tracey I. Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nat Med. 2010;16:1277–83. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.2229. 96. Varelmann D, Pancaro C, Cappiello EC, Camann WR. Nocebo-induced hyperalgesia during local anesthetic injection. Anesth Analg. 2010;110:868–70. https://doi.org/10.1213/ ANE.0b013e3181cc5727. 97. Vase L, Amanzio M, Price DD. Nocebo vs. placebo: the challenges of trial design in analgesia research. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2015;97:143–50. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpt.31. 98. Vase L, Robinson ME, Verne GN, Price DD. Increased placebo analgesia over time in irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) patients is associated with desire and expectation but not endogenous opioid mechanisms. Pain. 2005;115:338–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2005.03.014. 99. Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain. 1990;43:121–8. 100. Wager TD, Atlas LY. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:403–18. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3976. 101. Wager TD, Atlas LY, Leotti LA, Rilling JK. Predicting individual differences in placebo analgesia: contributions of brain activity during anticipation and pain experience. J Neurosci. 2011;31:439–52. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-10.2011. 102. Wager TD, Matre D, Casey KL. Placebo effects in laser-evoked pain potentials. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:219–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2006.01.007. 103. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303:1162–7. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1093065. 104. Wager TD, Scott DJ, Zubieta J-K. Placebo effects on human mu-opioid activity during pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:11056–61. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0702413104. 105. Wampold B. The therapeutic value of the relationship in the placebo effect and other healing practices, vol. 139; 2018. p. 191–210. 106. Wickramasekera I. A conditioned response model of the placebo effect predictions from the model. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1980;5:5–18. 107. Zubieta J-K. Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on -opioid receptors. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7754–62. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0439-05.2005. 108. Zubieta J-K, Stohler CS. Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:198–210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04424.x. 109. Zunhammer M, Ploner M, Engelbrecht C, Bock J, Kessner SS, Bingel U. The effects of treatment failure generalize across different routes of drug administration. Sci Transl Med. 2017;9:eaal2999. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aal2999. Chapter 5 The Special Case of High-Altitude Headache Diletta Barbiani, Eleonora Camerone, and Fabrizio Benedetti 5.1 High-Altitude Headache: Causes and Pathophysiology High-altitude headache is one of the many neurophysiological symptoms associated with the ascent to high altitudes and represents a core indicator of the clinical condition triggered by the drop in atmospheric oxygen pressure and by the decreased oxygen concentration in the air, known as acute mountain sickness (AMS) [10, 21]. AMS is a complex syndrome, defined as the combination of the presence of headache in an unacclimatized person who has recently reached an altitude above 2500 m along with one or more of such symptoms as gastrointestinal disturbances (anorexia, nausea, or vomiting), shortness of breath, dizziness, light headedness, insomnia, or fatigue. Symptoms could present as early as 1 h after having reached high altitude but usually develop within 6–10 h after ascent [14], and are generally assessed by means of the Lake Louise Score Questionnaire [19]. About 80% of people report headache as a main symptom when going to high altitudes, especially with rapid ascent and very high altitude [16]. When little oxygen is available in the air, a condition called hypoxia, several body functions counterbalance the oxygen shortage by triggering at least three fundamental compensatory responses: increase in ventilation (hyperventilation) through the activation of chemoreceptors; increase in circulation through increased cardiac output, e.g., heart rate increase; and increase in perfusion through vasodilation, e.g., cerebral vasodilation, whereby prostaglandins (PG) such as PGE2 have been found to be involved. Thus, the reduction of the D. Barbiani · F. Benedetti (*) Department of Neuroscience, University of Turin Medical School, Turin, Italy Plateau Rosà Laboratories, Plateau Rosà, Breuil-Cervinia, Italy Plateau Rosà Laboratories, Plateau Rosà, Zermatt, Switzerland e-mail: [email protected] E. Camerone Department of Neuroscience, University of Genoa, Genoa, Italy © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_5 57 58 D. Barbiani et al. partial pressure of oxygen in inspired air compromises the supply of oxygen to the tissues resulting in high-altitude headache and mountain sickness [14]. Despite the complex nature of this hypoxia-related clinical syndrome, headache is the cardinal symptom and indeed the most common symptom even at relatively low altitudes [14]. As far as we know, there are at least two pathways triggering high-altitude headache. The first is represented by the acute effects of hypoxia on PG synthesis through the cyclooxygenase (COX) enzyme, with the formation of PGD2, PGF2, PGE2, PGI2 (prostacyclin), and thromboxane (TX) A2 [2, 18]. One of the most important effects of these eicosanoids, particularly PGE2, is represented by vasodilation, which is thought to be the principal factor inducing acute hypoxia headache [7–9, 15, 17], although the direct stimulation of nociceptive afferents cannot be ruled out [12]. However, a second pathway involved in high-altitude headache is represented by the hypoxia-related hyperventilation that, in turn, induces the excessive elimination of carbon dioxide (CO2) with the consequent increase in blood pH (alkalosis) [20]. In keeping with the important role of alkalosis in AMS and high-altitude headache is the effect of blood pH reduction by means of acetazolamide [13]. Notwithstanding the therapeutic acidifying properties of acetazolamide, there are at least two other effective treatments for high-altitude headache: oxygen inhalation [1, 4] and oral cyclooxygenase inhibitors, such as aspirin [5, 6]. Whereas the former restores blood oxygen saturation (SO2), thus decreasing hyperventilation and alkalosis, the latter inhibits the hypoxia-activated cyclooxygenase and PG synthesis, thus reducing cerebral vasodilation [3]. In order to understand whether placebo effects have any role in these functions, that is, whether placebo effects occur for any physiological function that is involved in acute mountain sickness symptoms, what is needed is a model that is both ethical and amenable to scientific investigation. Indeed, needless to mention are the ethical constraints going hand in hand with the possible investigation of “anti-hypoxic” placebo effects in the clinical setting, in that we would need to study patients in hypoxic conditions, namely, with low blood oxygen saturation (SO2). For example, replacing even a tiny amount of oxygen with a placebo in a patient with either respiratory failure or admitted to an intensive care unit would certainly be unethical, and investigating the effects of placebo treatments on SO2 or impaired ventilation would be impracticable at the bedside. 5.2 The High-Altitude Model The majority of placebo and nocebo research has been conducted in experimental rather than clinical settings [4], and there are at least three reasons for this. First, there are less ethical issues when using experimental compared to clinical settings. Second, there are a limited number of protocols and measurements that can be used in patients compared to healthy volunteers. Third, the experimental setting allows to control for confounding variables, such as spontaneous remission and various methodological biases, including subject biases, co-interventions, and regression to the 5 The Special Case of High-Altitude Headache 59 mean [2]. For example, spontaneous remission refers to the natural improvement of the symptomatology mirroring the natural course of a disease or illness. In order to control for such confounder, research must include a control group, often called the “natural history” group, in which neither placebo nor true treatment is given to patients so that it can be representative of the natural course of the condition under investigation [2]. Ethical constraints restrain from including a natural history group in clinical trials, thus an experimental setting becomes necessary [2]. Although research relying on experimental setting has been proven to be extremely informative, this is limited, in that it prevents to extend our knowledge and understanding of placebo and nocebo mechanisms to the clinical population and to clinical conditions [2]. Within this context, the high-altitude, or hypobaric hypoxia, headache model acts as an extremely valuable resource because it exemplifies a borderline condition between the clinical and the experimental setting and allows to induce a clinical condition by bringing subjects from a region of high oxygen pressure (sea level, 159 mmHg) to a region of lower oxygen pressure (high altitude, e.g., 3500 m, 102 mmHg). It is a clinical model because subjects actually experience headache. However, this can be induced by simply bringing healthy volunteers to high altitude; that is to say, it is a model that allows us to study a clinical condition that is induced experimentally. As altitude increases, oxygen pressure drops, and in these extreme conditions, where both physical and cognitive performances decline very quickly, hypoxia triggers several compensatory responses, such as hyperventilation through the chemoreceptors, increased cardiac output through augmented heart rate, and increased brain perfusion through vasodilation. Basically, our body naturally reacts to oxygen shortage by means of compensatory strategies, the success of which strongly depends on the entity of the drop of oxygen pressure, in other words, on the altitude to which subjects go up to. In this regard, it is possible to differentiate between four high-altitude zones, depending on how the human body responds to oxygen shortage [11]. The zone from sea level to 1800–2000 m is called “indifferent,” as in most people there are no detectable effects on the body. The zone from 2000 to 4000 m is called “full compensation zone,” as many physiological responses compensate the low oxygen pressure, e.g., hyperventilation, increased heart rate, and cerebral vasodilation. The zone from 4000 to 7000 m is called “partial compensation zone,” where compensatory mechanisms do not fully cover the oxygen shortage and symptoms such as pulmonary edema and cerebral edema may occur. Finally, in the zone above 7000 m, the so-called death zone, oxygen is insufficient, compensatory mechanisms fail to compensate for the drop in atmospheric oxygen, and individuals are at high risk of death. Most of the studies at high altitude have been conducted in the “full compensation zone,” where oxygen availability is 64% compared to sea level. The reason for that is simple: we need a degree of hypoxia in which several physiological compensatory responses develop, yet they are not threatening for life. This is made possible by one of the locations where we work, namely, the Center for Hypoxia at the Plateau Rosà Laboratories, located at an altitude of 3500 m in the Matterhorn area at the border between Italy and Switzerland. In this zone, the abovementioned acute mountain sickness symptoms are often detected in association with compensatory 60 D. Barbiani et al. mechanisms which are capable of counterbalancing the low oxygen pressure. Among these, hyperventilation and vasodilation are thought to be at the core of the onset of altitude headache. 5.3 an Placebos and Nocebos Affect Headache at High C Altitude? The hypobaric hypoxia headache model allows to investigate the effect that placebos and nocebos may have not only on the subjectively reported headache pain but also on the neurobiological underpinnings [3–5]. Indeed, recent research has shown that placebos and nocebos modulate high-altitude headache and its physiological correlates [3–5]. For instance, in 2015, Benedetti et al. [4] investigated the effect of placebo oxygen on headache pain along with the assessment of physiological parameters [4]. Headache was assessed pre- and post-exercise by means of a numerical rating scale (NRS), and fatigue was also assessed, along with measures of SO2, heart rate (HR), and PGE2. Exercise consisted in subjects completing 3000 steps on a stepper. Oxygen placebo was given to participants via an oxygen mask connected to an oxygen supply with no real oxygen inside. Results from this study highlighted that placebo oxygen alone was effective in reducing fatigue; however no significant changes were reported in relation to headache at rest, post-exercise headache, HR, PGE2, and SO2 [4]. Differently, when placebo administration was preceded by oxygen preconditioning, whereby real oxygen was administered for several sessions in a row and then replaced by sham (placebo) oxygen on the last test session, not only fatigue was found to be reduced but also post-exercise headache, HR, and PGE2. However, no changes were observed for headache at rest as well as for SO2. The most interesting finding of this study is the influence that placebo administration had on peripheral mechanisms such as PGE2, though without any increase in SO2, showing how the placebo effect is not only a psychological but also a biological phenomenon. This notwithstanding, this study also sheds light on the limits of a placebo treatment, in that a placebo without a prior preconditioning procedure is incapable of affecting biological pathways [4]. Moreover, in no case a placebo had the power of reducing pre-exercise headache, suggesting the effectiveness of an “anti-headache placebo” if and only if headache is induced or enhanced by means of physical exercise [4]. In a different study, hypobaric hypoxia headache was investigated in the context of two different modalities of placebo administration, namely, placebo oxygen inhaled through an oxygen mask and placebo aspirin pills [3]. Headache and physiological parameters were first assessed at sea level (240 m) during the first day and then at 3500 m on the same day and on the two following days [3]. The following measurements were taken: headache, SO2, minute ventilation (Vmin), blood pH, salivary PGD2, PGE2, PGF2, PGI2, and TXA2. Performance was evaluated by asking subjects to complete 3000 steps on a stepper. Participants were divided into four 5 The Special Case of High-Altitude Headache 61 groups: (a) no treatment; (b) placebo oxygen, with real oxygen preconditioning; (c) placebo aspirin, with real aspirin preconditioning; and (d) “mixed” placebo, in which preconditioning with oxygen was followed by the administration of placebo aspirin. The oxygen placebo group showed reduction of headache, Vmin, blood alkalosis, and PGE2, yet with no increase in SO2. Differently, in the placebo aspirin group, reduction of headache pain was associated with the inhibition of all products of cyclooxygenase, PGD2, PGE2, PGF2, PGI2, and TXA2, whereas ventilation and blood alkalosis were not affected. These results seem to both corroborate and extend what was previously shown by Benedetti and his team in 2015; they emphasize how the placebo effect is not a unitary phenomenon: different placebos interact and affect different physiological pathways; specifically, placebo oxygen influences the ventilation-alkalosis pathway, whereas placebo aspirin affects the COX-PG pathway [3]. One of the core strengths of the hypobaric hypoxia model is that it allows to investigate not only the placebo but also the nocebo effect, though still without encountering important ethical limits. For example, Benedetti et al. [5] investigated nocebo and placebo effects in high-altitude headache and at the level of the COX-PG pathway. A model of interindividual communication was used to induce nocebo responses. Precisely, one subject, the “trigger,” was informed by the researchers of the likelihood of experiencing severe headache because of the lower oxygen concentration at high altitude. In the following weeks, the researchers were contacted by other participants of the study asking for more information about the possibility of experiencing high-altitude headache. This indicated that the trigger subject had spread the information across some subjects, who then spread the information across others, and so on. The experimental group consisted of subjects who were informed about the risk of headache by the trigger, whereas the control group consisted of subjects who were unaware of such risk. An increase of headache pain and salivary COX products (PGD2, PGE2, PGF2, PGI2, TXA2) was found in the nocebo compared to the control group [5]. In addition, aspirin placebo was given to all subjects suffering from headache, both in the control and in the experimental group. It was shown that placebo administration ameliorated headache pain and inhibited prostaglandins synthesis in headache sufferers in the experimental group but not in the control group. In other words, both aspirin and placebo reduced pain in the socially affected individuals. The difference in the placebo response between the two groups is ascribable to the different baseline values of headache pain and prostaglandins—e.g., PGE2—induced by the dissemination of negative information. A placebo effect was observed only for the “infected” individuals who found themselves at an underprivileged nocebo baseline of increased pain, with the placebo acting as a neutralizer of the nocebo component of the prostaglandin and pain increase. Therefore, this study suggests that placebos could be effective only on the nocebo-related component of pain. It is worth noting how this study again shows that, similarly to a placebo, also a nocebo is both a psychological and biological phenomenon capable of modulating different biochemical pathways involved in headache pain. 62 5.4 D. Barbiani et al. Conclusions Research at high altitude has proven itself to be an extraordinary model, useful to overcome some of the key issues in placebo and nocebo studies. One of the main boons of this approach is that it permits to create a borderline condition between the experimental and the clinical setting, thus allowing us to study a clinical condition which is induced experimentally by simply bringing subjects to high altitude. Moreover, high-altitude research has given a significant contribution to the development of our understanding of placebo and nocebo phenomena, showing, for instance, how fatigue is very sensitive to a placebo alone, without the requirement of a preconditioning procedure. This suggests that positive expectations induced by positive verbal suggestions alone are sufficient to trigger a placebo response in those more “psychologically susceptible” parameters such as fatigue. On the other hand, a preconditioning procedure prior to placebo administration seems to be necessary in order to observe changes in physiological parameters—ventilation, blood pH, heart activity, prostaglandins, and TXA2—as well as in post-exercise headache. Interestingly, such changes occur independently from the variation of SO2. These findings suggest that learning mechanisms play a crucial role in placebo responses and often represent the necessary condition in order to have a placebo response. One of the most intriguing aspects of this line of research is the effect that a placebo has on an array of different parameters despite no changes in SO2, suggesting that psychobiological mechanisms may, at times, be as important as the oxygen level in the body. Moreover, there is growing evidence showing how placebo responses vary depending on the type of placebo administered: placebo aspirin and placebo oxygen interact and affect different physiological pathways, suggesting that the placebo effect is not a unitary phenomenon, affecting both psychological and physiological parameters. In addition, nocebo mechanisms have been shown to act in a similar manner as placebo mechanisms. For example, in headache pain, negative expectations lead to changes both at the psychological level, such as increased headache perception, and at the physiological level, such as increased salivary COX products. Taken all together, these findings indicate how the biological factor alone, that is hypobaric hypoxia, is only partially responsible for the changes observed at high altitude. Indeed, psychological factors (expectations) and learning mechanisms (conditioning) seem to play a fundamental role in the amelioration (placebo) or generation (nocebo) of symptoms, as well as in the biochemical changes associated to hypoxia. Despite the large contribution that the high-altitude model has offered so far, further research is required for a more in-depth understanding of this complex and multiform area of investigation. First, research must address the question of how physiological placebo responses occur and how they manage to bypass the oxygen shortage in the body. Second, research should investigate other body functions that may be affected by placebos and explore other physiological and biochemical 5 The Special Case of High-Altitude Headache 63 parameters. Third, it may be challenging to study the extent to which placebo responses can push themselves forward, for example, by investigating what happens with oxygen reductions that go beyond 50%. References 1. Bartsch P, Baumgartner RW, Waber U, Maggiorini M, Oelz O. Comparison of carbondioxideenriched, oxygen-enriched, and normal air in treatment of acute mountain sickness. Lancet. 1990;336:772–5. 2. Benedetti F. Perspective placebo effects: from the neurobiological paradigm to translational implications. Neuron. 2014;84:623–37. 3. Benedetti F, Dogue S. Different placebos, different mechanisms, different outcomes: lessons for clinical trials. PLoS One. 2015;10(11):e0140967. 4. Benedetti F, Durando J, Giudetti L, Pampallona A, Vighetti S. High altitude headache: the effects of real versus sham oxygen administration. Pain. 2015;156:2326–36. 5. Benedetti F, Durando J, Vighetti S. Nocebo and placebo modulation of hypobaric hypoxia headache involves the cyclooxygenase-prostaglandins pathway. Pain. 2014;155(5):921–8. 6. Burtscher M, Likar R, Nachbauer W, Philadelphy M. Aspirin for prophylaxis against headache at high altitudes: randomised, double blind, placebo controlled trial. Br Med J. 1998;316:1057–8. 7. Busse R, Fosterman U, Matsuda H, Pohl U. The role of prostaglandins in the endotheliummediated vasodilatory response to hypoxia. Pflugers Arch. 1984;401:77–83. 8. Davis RJ, Murdoch CE, Ali M, Purbrick S, Ravid R, Baxter GS, et al. EP4 prostanoid receptormediated vasodilation of human middle cerebral arteries. Br J Pharmacol. 2004;141:580–5. 9. Fredericks KT, Liu Y, Rusch NJ, Lombard JH. Role of endothelium and arterial K+ channels in mediating hypoxic dilation of middle cerebral arteries. Am J Physiol. 1994;267:H580–6. 10. Imray C, Wright A, Subudhi A, Roach R. Acute mountain sickness: pathophysiology, prevention and treatment. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2010;52:467–84. 11. International Society for Mountain Medicine. Non-Physician Altitude Tutorial. 2005. Archived from the original on 2011-06-06. Retrieved 22 Dec 2005. 12. Kawabata A. Prostaglandin E2 and pain—an update. Biol Pharm Bull. 2011;34:1170–3. 13. Leaf DE, Goldfarb DS. Mechanisms of action of acetazolamide in the prophylaxis and treatment of acute mountain sickness. J Appl Physiol. 2007;102(1):313–22. 14. Marmura MJ, Hernandez PB. High-altitude headache. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2015;19(5):483. 15. Messina EJ, Sun D, Koller A, Wolin MS, Kaley G. Role of endothelium-derived prostaglandins in hypoxia elicited arteriolar dilation in rat skeletal muscle. Circ Res. 1992;71:790–6. 16. Porcelli MJ, Gugelchuk GM. A trek to the top: a review of acute mountain sickness. J Am Osteopath Assoc. 1995;95:718–20. 17. Ray CJ, Abbas MR, Coney AM, Marshall JM. Interactions of adenosine, prostaglandins and nitric oxide in hypoxia-induced vasodilatation: in vivo and in nitro studies. J Physiol. 2002;544:195–209. 18. Richalet JP, Hornych A, Rathat C, Aumont J, Larmignat P, Rémy P. Plasma prostaglandins, leukotrienes and thromboxane in acute high altitude hypoxia. Respir Physiol. 1991;85:205–15. 19. Sutton JR, Coates G, Houston CS, Oelz O. The Lake Louise consensus on the definition and quantification of altitude illness. In: Sutton JR, Coates G, Houston CS, editors. Hypoxia and mountain medicine. Burlington: Queen City Printers; 1992. p. 327–30. 20. West JB. The physiologic basis of high-altitude diseases. Ann Intern Med. 2004;141:789–800. 21. Wilson MH, Newman S, Imray CS. The cerebral effects of ascent to high altitudes. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:175–91. Chapter 6 Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache Jakob Møller Hansen and Messoud Ashina 6.1 Introduction Migraine is a complex neurovascular disorder characterized by recurrent episodes of headache with associated features [14] and is one of the most prevalent [32] and disabling neurological disorders [23]. A key feature of migraine is that various factors can trigger an attack, and this phenomenon provides a unique opportunity to investigate disease mechanisms by experimentally inducing migraine attacks [6]. Because migraine attacks are fully reversible and amendable to therapy, the headache- or migraine-provoking property of endogenous signaling molecules can be tested in a human model which offers unique possibilities to study mechanisms responsible for migraine and to explore the mechanisms of action of existing and future anti-migraine drugs [25]. Like all other interventions, participant expectations, content and delivery of information, and unspecific effects of the intervention may exert a placebo or nocebo effect on the outcomes. In this chapter, we summarize the recorded placebo responses from human experimental headache studies and discuss how placebo responses may be mitigated in future experimental human migraine studies. J. M. Hansen · M. Ashina (*) Department of Neurology, Danish Headache Center, Rigshospitalet, Glostrup, Denmark Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_6 65 66 6.2 J. M. Hansen and M. Ashina he Rationale for Using Human Experimental Migraine T Studies In vitro studies have contributed in the characterization of receptors in cranial blood vessels and the identification of new possible anti-migraine agents. Animal models enable the study of vascular responses, neurogenic inflammation, and peptide release and thus provided leads in the search for migraine mechanisms. So far, however, animal models cannot predict the efficacy of new therapies for migraine. Human models of migraine can be used to test whether endogenous signaling molecules or other putative trigger factors provoke migraine in humans; provocation of an attack suggests involvement of the triggering factor in the mechanisms of spontaneous migraine attack. Experience has shown that if a factor induces migraine in patients with migraine, the same factor will also induce a milder headache in people without migraine; therefore, a first step in establishing a migraine-inducing effect is to test for any headache-eliciting effect in healthy volunteers. The fact that patients with migraine are more prone than healthy volunteers to experience migraine attacks in response to these triggering factors suggests the existence of a migraine threshold phenomenon [10, 36]. Experimental pain studies based on well-controlled study conditions and uniform outcome assessments are necessary to increase our understanding of basic disease processes. The subjects may well be subject to placebo effects, caused by conditioning, expectations, and the endogenous opioid system. This should be taken into account when designing human provocation studies. Provocation studies can be tailored to examine specific hypotheses; if the focus of the study is to identify imaging or biochemical markers of migraine, scans and blood samples are collected at baseline, at predefined intervals during the study when effects are expected, and after treatment intervention (Fig. 6.1). With the addition of other imaging modalities such as high-field MRI, even more detailed information can be collected on the vascular response of the cephalic circulation. 6.3 Placebo Response in Human Experimental Models For this chapter we selected experimental human studies where a placebo group was included. Migraine provocation studies should preferably be double-blind crossover studies and typically involve random assignment of participants to receive intravenous infusion of either the putative triggering factor or a placebo. This is also helpful for blinding purposes. The placebo effects reported in human experimental studies are summarized in Table 6.1. It is notable that both controls and patients report headache and also migraine after provocation with placebo. Mostly, however, the placebo response is modest. 6 Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache 67 Fig. 6.1 The human provocation model, modified from [25]. In the main version of this model, patients with migraine are randomly allocated to receive intravenous infusion (25 min) of “target substance” or placebo (isotonic saline) in a double-blind, crossover design. Headache intensity is recorded on a verbal rating scale from 0 to 10 (0, no headache; 1, a very mild headache (including a feeling of pressing or throbbing); 5, moderate headache; 10, worst imaginable headache). At predefined intervals hemodynamic variables are recorded (mean velocity of blood flow in the middle cerebral artery by transcranial Doppler with hand-held 2-MHz probes; diameter of the frontal branch of the superficial temporal artery by a high-resolution ultrasonography unit). Data on headache occurrence are captured in a headache diary [28]; headache scores are filled in during the infusion and hourly after discharge. All characteristics of headache and symptoms that accompany it are recorded in the diary. The diary included headache characteristics and accompanying symptoms necessary to classify the headache according to ICHD-III 6.4 lacebo in Relation to Vascular Effects in Human P Experimental Studies Arterial dilatation may cause headache [24], and glyceryl trinitrate (GTN) infusion does cause a more pronounced dilation of extra- and intracerebral arteries in migraine patients than in controls [33]. In a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-­blind, crossover study, 1.5-T magnetic resonance angiogram (MRA) was combined with GTN infusion in healthy volunteers, to report a vasodilation of the middle cerebral artery (MCA) that was significantly larger after GTN than placebo. Still, placebo led to a 1–2% increase in diameter and cross-sectional area [12]. The vascular effects of calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP) infusion in healthy volunteers were investigated with high-resolution 3-T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI): CGRP causes significant dilation of the extracerebral middle meningeal artery (MMA) but not of the intracerebral MCA compared with placebo [3]. 68 J. M. Hansen and M. Ashina Table 6.1 Human experimental headache studies of various signaling molecules, where a placebo group was included (selected) Compound Design Glyceryl Double-blind, trinitrate (GTN) placebo-­ controlled, crossover trial Study population Migraine n = 12 Glyceryl Randomized, trinitrate (GTN) double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Chronic tension-­ type headache n = 16 Glyceryl Double-blind, trinitrate (GTN) placebo-­ controlled, crossover trial Migraine n = 28 Carbachol Controls n = 12 Carbachol Vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Migraine n = 18 Controls Randomized, n = 12 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Headache reported (active vs. placebo) Migraine: 8/10 (80%) after GTN vs. 1/12 (8%) after placebo Delayed headache: 15/16 (94%) after GTN vs. 11/16 (69%) after placebo Migraine: 16 (57.1%) after GTN vs. 0% after placebo Comment Ref. Thomsen et al. [34] Area under the headache curve larger after GTN than after placebo Ashina et al. [5] Perrotta GTN (0.9 mg et al. [26] sublingual) GTN leads to facilitation of the temporal processing of the nociceptive inputs at spinal level, without a notable placebo effect Schytz Headache: et al. [31] 9 (75%) after carbachol vs. 3 (25%) after placebo Schytz Marked placebo Headache: response precluded et al. [30] 15 (83%) further studies in after carbachol vs. carbachol 8 (44%) after placebo Migraine: 8 (44%) after carbachol vs. 6 (33%) after placebo Hansen Side effects Headache: et al. [11] 7 (58%) after Heat sensation VIP: 10/12 VIP vs. 3 (25%) after Placebo 3/12 placebo 6 Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache 69 Table 6.1 (continued) Compound Vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) PDE 3 (Cilostazol) Design Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Study population Migraine n = 12 Controls n = 12 PDE 3 (Cilostazol) Migraine Randomized, n = 14 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study PDE 5 (Sildenafil) Controls Randomized, n = 10 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study PDE 5 (Sildenafil) Migraine Randomized, n = 12 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP) Migraine Randomized, n = 12 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Headache reported (active vs. placebo) Headache: 10 (83%) after VIP vs. 4 (33%) after placebo Headache: 11 (92%) after cilostazol vs. 3 (25%) after placebo Migraine: 12 (86%) after cilostazol vs. 2 (14%) after placebo Comment Side effects Heat sensation VIP: 12/12 Placebo 3/12 Patients reported that the attacks mimicked their usual migraine attacks and that cilostazol-induced attacks responded to their usual migraine treatment Headache and Headache: 9 (90%) after migraine caused by sildenafil vs. sildenafil was not 3 (30%) after accompanied by changes in plasma placebo levels of CGRP, cGMP, and cAMP compared to placebo [17] Delayed migraine-­ Migraine: 10/12 (83%) like headache without initial after sildenafil vs. dilatation of the middle cerebral 2/12 (16%) after placebo artery Headache: 12 (100%) after CGRP vs. 1 (8%) after placebo Migraine: 3 (33%) after CGRP vs. 0 (0%) after placebo Ref. Rahmann et al. [27] Birk et al. [7] Guo et al. [9] Kruuse et al. [19] Kruuse et al. [18] Lassen et al. [20] (continued) 70 J. M. Hansen and M. Ashina Table 6.1 (continued) Compound Hypoxia Study Design population Migraine Randomized, n = 15 double-blind, sham-­ controlled crossover study Pituitary adenylate cyclase-­ activating polypeptide (PACAP-38) Controls Randomized, n = 12 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Pituitary adenylate cyclase-­ activating polypeptide (PACAP-38) Migraine Randomized, n = 12 double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Prostaglandin I2 Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Prostaglandin I2 Controls n = 12 Migraine n = 12 Headache reported (active vs. placebo) Migraine: 8 (53%) during hypoxia vs. 1 (7%) during sham Comment Two patients experienced migraine without aura-like attacks 5 and 8 h after initiation of sham procedure Side effects: Headache: Heat sensation 12 (100%) after PACAP PACAP: 11/12 vs. 2 (17%) Placebo 4/12 after placebo Migraine: 2 (17%) after PACAP vs. 0 (0%) after placebo PACAP38 infusion Headache: caused headache, 11 (92%) after PACAP vasodilatation, and vs. 3 (25%) delayed migraine-­ after placebo like attacks Heat sensation Migraine: 7 (58%) after PACAP: 12/12 PACAP vs. 0 Placebo 0/12 (0%) after placebo Area under the Headache: headache curve 11 (92%) after PGI(2) larger after PGI2 than after placebo vs. 0 after placebo Area under the Headache: headache curve 12 (100%) after PGI(2) larger after PGI2 vs. 3 (25%) than after placebo after placebo Migraine: 6 (50%) after PGI(2) vs. 2 (17%) after placebo Ref. Arngrim et al. [2] Schytz et al. [29] Schytz et al. [29] Wienecke et al. [38] Wienecke et al. [37] 6 Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache 71 Table 6.1 (continued) Compound Prostaglandin E2 Prostaglandin E2 6.5 Design Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Randomized, double-blind, placebo-­ controlled crossover study Study population Controls n = 11 Migraine n = 12 Headache reported (active vs. placebo) Headache: 11 (100%) after PGE(2) vs. 0 after placebo Migraine: 9 (75%) after PGE(2) vs. 0 after placebo Comment Area under the headache curve larger after PGE2 than after placebo Ref. Wienecke et al. [39] Antonova Migraine-like attacks during, and et al. [1] immediately after, the PGE(2) infusion contrast with those found in previous provocation studies revention of Experimentally Induced Headache P and Migraine Tvedskov and colleagues conducted two randomized placebo-controlled crossover studies and found that valproate [35] and propranolol [36] had no effect on GTN-­ induced headache and migraine. The ineffectiveness of a widely used migraine prophylactics limits the usefulness of the model and must be considered in future testing of new migraine prophylactic and acute drugs [4]. Placebo response rate for headache relief in migraine trials is considerable [21] but varies considerably from 6% [15] to 47% [8], with a reported mean of 29% in a meta-analysis of 98 studies [22]. This observation underscores the importance of including an adequate control group and blinding in all clinical trials of interventions for migraine. 6.6 Can the Placebo Response Be Mitigated? In a large Cochrane review of placebo interventions, the authors concluded that there is no evidence that placebo interventions in general have clinically important effects. They also suggested that placebo interventions can influence patient-­ reported outcomes, especially pain and nausea, though it is difficult to distinguish patient-reported effects of placebo from biased reporting. Variations in the effect of placebo were partly explained by variations in how trials were conducted and how patients were informed [13]. 72 J. M. Hansen and M. Ashina This was also the subject for an elegant study that examined whether positive information about active medication was a contributing factor in successful migraine treatment [16]. In six subsequent migraine attacks, migraine patients were given either placebo or active treatment, administered under three information conditions ranging from negative to neutral to positive. Treatment order was randomized. Active treatment was superior to placebo, and the efficacy of open-label placebo was superior to that of no treatment. Relative to no treatment, the placebo, under each information condition, accounted for more than 50% of the drug effect. Increasing “positive” information incrementally boosted the efficacy of both placebo and medication during migraine attacks. The benefits of placebo persisted even if placebo was honestly described. 6.7 Conclusion and Implications for Study Design Human experimental migraine studies are in many ways not different from migraine treatment trials: An investigator is testing the effect of an experimental intervention. As such, it is to be expected that this intervention will yield some placebo responses. Placebo responses are indeed found in experimental human migraine studies: We report a modest placebo effect. With the use of an appropriate control intervention, this effect may be mitigated and taken into account. We suggest that care should be taken to minimize unspecific effects including placebo by standardizing experimental conditions. References 1. Antonova M, Wienecke T, Olesen J, Ashina M. Prostaglandin E(2) induces immediate migraine-like attack in migraine patients without aura. Cephalalgia. 2012;32:822–33. 2. Arngrim N, Schytz HW, Britze J, Amin FM, Vestergaard MB, Hougaard A, Wolfram F, de Koning PJ, Olsen KS, Secher NH, Larsson HB, Olesen J, Ashina M. Migraine induced by hypoxia: an MRI spectroscopy and angiography study. Brain. 2016;139:723–37. 3. Asghar MS, Hansen AE, Kapijimpanga T, van der Geest RJ, van der Koning P, Larsson HB, Olesen J, Ashina M. Dilation by CGRP of middle meningeal artery and reversal by sumatriptan in normal volunteers. Neurology. 2010;75:1520–6. 4. Ashina M. Human models—screening models. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2008. 5. Ashina M, Bendtsen L, Jensen R, Olesen J. Nitric oxide-induced headache in patients with chronic tension-type headache. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 9):1830–7. 6. Ashina M, Hansen JM, Bo AD, Olesen J. Human models of migraine—short-term pain for long-term gain. Nat Rev Neurol. 2017;13:713–24. 7. Birk S, Kruuse C, Petersen KA, Tfelt-Hansen P, Olesen J. The headache-inducing effect of cilostazol in human volunteers. Cephalalgia. 2006;26:1304–9. 8. Diener HC, Dowson AJ, Ferrari M, Nappi G, Tfelt-Hansen P. Unbalanced randomization influences placebo response: scientific versus ethical issues around the use of placebo in migraine trials. Cephalalgia. 1999;19:699–700. 6 Placebo Response in Human Models of Headache 73 9. Guo S, Olesen J, Ashina M. Phosphodiesterase 3 inhibitor cilostazol induces migraine-like attacks via cyclic AMP increase. Brain. 2014;137:2951–9. 10. Hansen EK, Olesen J. Towards a pragmatic human migraine model for drug testing: 2. Isosorbide-5-mononitrate in healthy individuals. Cephalalgia. 2017;37(1):11–9. 11. Hansen JM, Sitarz J, Birk S, Rahmann AM, Oturai PS, Fahrenkrug J, Olesen J, Ashina M. Vasoactive intestinal polypeptide evokes only a minimal headache in healthy volunteers. Cephalalgia. 2006;26:992–1003. 12. Hansen JM, Pedersen D, Larsen VA, Sanchez-del-Rio M, Alvarez Linera JR, Olesen J, Ashina M. Magnetic resonance angiography shows dilatation of the middle cerebral artery after infusion of glyceryl trinitrate in healthy volunteers. Cephalalgia. 2007;27:118–27. 13. Hrobjartsson A, Gotzsche PC. Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010;(1):CD003974. 14. IHS. The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition (beta version). Cephalalgia. 2013;33:629–808. 15. Jensen K, Tfelt-Hansen P, Hansen EW, Krois EH, Pedersen OS. Introduction of a novel self-injector for sumatriptan. A controlled clinical trial in general practice. Cephalalgia. 1995;15:423–9. 16. Kam-Hansen S, Jakubowski M, Kelley JM, Kirsch I, Hoaglin DC, Kaptchuk TJ, Burstein R. Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6:218ra5. 17. Kruuse C, Frandsen E, Schifter S, Thomsen LL, Birk S, Olesen J. Plasma levels of cAMP, cGMP and CGRP in sildenafil-induced headache. Cephalalgia. 2004;24:547–53. 18. Kruuse C, Thomsen LL, Birk S, Olesen J. Migraine can be induced by sildenafil without changes in middle cerebral artery diameter. Brain. 2003;126:241–7. 19. Kruuse C, Thomsen LL, Jacobsen TB, Olesen J. The phosphodiesterase 5 inhibitor sildenafil has no effect on cerebral blood flow or blood velocity, but nevertheless induces headache in healthy subjects. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2002;22:1124–31. 20. Lassen LH, Haderslev PA, Jacobsen VB, Iversen HK, Sperling B, Olesen J. CGRP may play a causative role in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2002;22:54–61. 21. Loder E, Goldstein R, Biondi D. Placebo effects in oral triptan trials: the scientific and ethical rationale for continued use of placebo controls. Cephalalgia. 2005;25:124–31. 22. Macedo A, Farre M, Banos JE. A meta-analysis of the placebo response in acute migraine and how this response may be influenced by some of the characteristics of clinical trials. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2006;62:161–72. 23. Murray CJ, Vos T, Lozano R, Naghavi M, Flaxman AD, Michaud C, Ezzati M, Shibuya K, Salomon JA, Abdalla S, Aboyans V, Abraham J, Ackerman I, Aggarwal R, Ahn SY, Ali MK, Alvarado M, Anderson HR, Anderson LM, Andrews KG, Atkinson C, Baddour LM, Bahalim AN, Barker-Collo S, Barrero LH, Bartels DH, Basanez MG, Baxter A, Bell ML, Benjamin EJ, Bennett D, Bernabe E, Bhalla K, Bhandari B, Bikbov B, Bin Abdulhak A, Birbeck G, Black JA, Blencowe H, Blore JD, Blyth F, Bolliger I, Bonaventure A, Boufous S, Bourne R, Boussinesq M, Braithwaite T, Brayne C, Bridgett L, Brooker S, Brooks P, Brugha TS, Bryan-­ Hancock C, Bucello C, Buchbinder R, Buckle G, Budke CM, Burch M, Burney P, Burstein R, Calabria B, Campbell B, Canter CE, Carabin H, Carapetis J, Carmona L, Cella C, Charlson F, Chen H, Cheng AT, Chou D, Chugh SS, Coffeng LE, Colan SD, Colquhoun S, Colson KE, Condon J, Connor MD, Cooper LT, Corriere M, Cortinovis M, de Vaccaro KC, Couser W, Cowie BC, Criqui MH, Cross M, Dabhadkar KC, Dahiya M, Dahodwala N, Damsere-Derry J, Danaei G, Davis A, De Leo D, Degenhardt L, Dellavalle R, Delossantos A, Denenberg J, Derrett S, Des Jarlais DC, Dharmaratne SD, et al. Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2012;380:2197–223. 24. Nichols FT 3rd, Mawad M, Mohr JP, Stein B, Hilal S, Michelsen WJ. Focal headache during balloon inflation in the internal carotid and middle cerebral arteries. Stroke. 1990;21: 555–9. 74 J. M. Hansen and M. Ashina 25. Olesen J, Tfelt-Hansen P, Ashina M. Finding new drug targets for the treatment of migraine attacks. Cephalalgia. 2009;29:909–20. 26. Perrotta A, Serrao M, Tassorelli C, Arce-Leal N, Guaschino E, Sances G, Rossi P, Bartolo M, Pierelli F, Sandrini G, Nappi G. Oral nitric-oxide donor glyceryl-trinitrate induces s­ ensitization in spinal cord pain processing in migraineurs: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-­over study. Eur J Pain. 2011;15:482–90. 27. Rahmann A, Wienecke T, Hansen JM, Fahrenkrug J, Olesen J, Ashina M. Vasoactive intestinal peptide causes marked cephalic vasodilation, but does not induce migraine. Cephalalgia. 2008;28:226–36. 28. Russell MB, Rasmussen BK, Brennum J, Iversen HK, Jensen RA, Olesen J. Presentation of a new instrument: the diagnostic headache diary. Cephalalgia. 1992;12:369–74. 29. Schytz HW, Birk S, Wienecke T, Kruuse C, Olesen J, Ashina M. PACAP38 induces migraine-­ like attacks in patients with migraine without aura. Brain. 2009;132:16–25. 30. Schytz HW, Wienecke T, Olesen J, Ashina M. Carbachol induces headache, but not migraine-­ like attacks, in patients with migraine without aura. Cephalalgia. 2010;30:337–45. 31. Schytz HW, Wienecke T, Oturai PS, Olesen J, Ashina M. The cholinomimetic agent carbachol induces headache in healthy subjects. Cephalalgia. 2009;29:258–68. 32. GBD 2015 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 diseases and injuries, 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. Lancet. 2016;388:1545–602. 33. Thomsen LL, Iversen HK, Brinck TA, Olesen J. Arterial supersensitivity to nitric oxide (nitroglycerin) in migraine sufferers. Cephalalgia. 1993;13:395–9; discussion 376. 34. Thomsen LL, Kruuse C, Iversen HK, Olesen J. A nitric oxide donor (nitroglycerin) triggers genuine migraine attacks. Eur J Neurol. 1994;1:73–80. 35. Tvedskov JF, Thomsen LL, Iversen HK, Gibson A, Wiliams P, Olesen J. The prophylactic effect of valproate on glyceryltrinitrate induced migraine. Cephalalgia. 2004;24:576–85. 36. Tvedskov JF, Thomsen LL, Iversen HK, Williams P, Gibson A, Jenkins K, Peck R, Olesen J. The effect of propranolol on glyceryltrinitrate-induced headache and arterial response. Cephalalgia. 2004;24:1076–87. 37. Wienecke T, Olesen J, Ashina M. Prostaglandin I2 (epoprostenol) triggers migraine-like attacks in migraineurs. Cephalalgia. 2010;30:179–90. 38. Wienecke T, Olesen J, Oturai PS, Ashina M. Prostacyclin (epoprostenol) induces headache in healthy subjects. Pain. 2008;139:106–16. 39. Wienecke T, Olesen J, Oturai PS, Ashina M. Prostaglandin E2(PGE2) induces headache in healthy subjects. Cephalalgia. 2009;29:509–19. Chapter 7 Nocebo in Headache Treatment Christina Deligianni and Dimos D. Mitsikostas 7.1 Introduction Among primary headache disorders, migraine and tension-type headache (TTH) are the most frequent with 1 year prevalence of 35 and 38%, respectively [36], affecting 1.04 and 1.89 billion of people worldwide (the third and sixth most prevalent conditions among all medical ones, respectively) [11]. Additionally, migraine is rating as the second leading condition causing disability after low back pain [11]. Although rare relatively (1‰), cluster headache damages severely personal life, becoming the third most important primary headache [32]. All three conditions are treatable, but due to safety and tolerability reasons, available preventive treatments have often limited success, even in the right hands. One out of five patients treated with any migraine preventive pharmaceutical agent will discontinue treatment because of tolerability and safety reasons [13]. Moreover, adherence is poor in migraine preventive treatments, as in most conditions requiring chronic therapy. Only one out of four patients complies with treatment in chronic migraine when treatment is required for 6 months, and this decreases to one out of five when treatment duration increases to 1 year [15]. Adherence seems similar among the oral migraine preventative drugs except perhaps for amitriptyline, nortriptyline, gabapentin, and divalproex, which show significantly lower odds of adherence when compared to topiramate; on the other hand, citalopram, fluoxetine, venlafaxine, atenolol, and metoprolol show a trend toward an increased likelihood of adherence compared to topiramate [15]. Other studies suggest that low adherence may be attributed to a number of factors, including side effects and/or lack of efficacy of the drug [16]. Previous experiences, personality, and a variety of cofactors modulate nocebo, even the price of treatment [37, 38]. Pharmacophobia and nocebo are C. Deligianni · D. D. Mitsikostas (*) 1st Neurology Department, Aeginition Hospital, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_7 75 76 C. Deligianni and D. D. Mitsikostas additional cofactors that border adherence and treatment outcomes significantly [21, 22]. Pharmacophobia refers to the irrational fear of medication, and nocebo refers to the experience of adverse events related to patients’ negative expectation that a treatment will harm most likely instead of help [21]. The frequency of adverse events can dramatically increase by informing patients about the possible side effects of the treatment and by negative expectations on the part of the patient [4, 5]. Patients who were told that they might experience sexual side effects after treatment with β-blocker drugs reported these symptoms between three and four times more often than patients in a control group who were not informed about these symptoms [34]. Nocebo varies by the condition, and it has been estimated in several brain disorders such as migraine [1, 26], epilepsy [39, 40], fibromyalgia [25], multiple sclerosis [30], Parkinson’s disease [35], neuropathic pain [29], and restless syndrome [33] and in patients with depression [10, 20, 27]. Within this context, all available data on nocebo related to headache treatment is summarized and discussed in this chapter. 7.2 ethodology to Estimate Nocebos in RCTs for Primary M Headaches Several researchers systematically searched for AEs in placebo arms in RCTs for primary headache without using the term nocebo [1, 18, 31]. One important finding was that the AEs in the placebo arms corresponded to those of the anti-migraine medication against which the placebo was compared [1]. Additionally, those studies showed the way to calculate nocebo in RCTs. In a following study, two specific outcomes were used to assess nocebo in RCTs: nocebo AEs (meaning the percentage of patients treated with placebo and experienced at least one AE) and nocebo dropouts (referring to the percentage of patients treated with placebo and discontinued treatment because of AE) [30]. These outcomes may represent the most pragmatic and clinically relevant ones for nocebo in RCTs and used in all subsequent meta-analyses to estimate nocebo in several neurological and pain conditions [14, 20, 28, 33, 39]. 7.3 Nocebos in RCTs for Primary Headaches Reuter and colleagues [31] first investigated nocebo in RCTs for headaches and found that up to one-third of migraineurs treated with placebo experience AEs. In trials for symptomatic migraine treatment with triptans, one out of five placebotreated patients reported at least one adverse event. The adverse events were grouped into three categories: migraine-related (such as nausea, photophobia, and phonophobia), drug-related (symptoms typical of the experimental compound such as 7 Nocebo in Headache Treatment 77 chest pressure in response to triptans), and nonspecific or coincidental (e.g., sleep disturbance). The adverse events in the placebo group were related to the drug under study and to the symptomatology of migraine, whereas some others had no obvious relation to the condition or treatment [31]. In another review aimed at estimating the placebo response in migraineurs treated with oral triptans, it was found that almost one out of four participants treated with placebo reported AEs. Fascinatingly, studies performed in North America showed a higher nocebo frequency than those conducted in Europe [18]. Amanzio and colleagues [1] published an extensive systematic review of nocebo in clinical trials for migraine. This was the first attempt to intensely investigate migraine-related nocebo effects. They investigated the adverse events after placebo in RCTs testing NSAIDs, triptans, or anticonvulsants. Their major finding was that nocebo AEs mirrored the adverse events expected of the active medication studied precisely. For example, anorexia and memory difficulties, which are typical AEs of anticonvulsants, were present only in the placebo arm of these trials. In other words, nocebo in migraine trials arose from patients’ distrust. This important meta-analysis aimed to investigate the mechanisms of nocebo in particular, rather than to investigate the magnitude of nocebo in RCTs for migraine, and migraine most likely was used as a vehicle pain condition in this study. For instance, the investigators searched RCTs for migraine trials, both symptomatic and preventive, only if specific anti-migraine agents were tested. Undoubtedly, the results of this meta-analysis confirmed their findings derived from experimental human studies that expectations modulate both nocebo and placebo (the expectation theory of placebo and nocebo) [5]. A meta-analysis of RCTs for all primary headache disorders [26], for both symptomatic and preventative treatments, including all medications used, revealed that the nocebo AE and nocebo dropouts frequencies for symptomatic migraine treatment trials were 18.45% (95% CI 14.90–22.23%) and 0.33% (95% CI 0.19–0.53%), respectively. In trials studying triptans exclusively, nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout frequencies were 20.93% (95% CI 16.46–25.78%) and 0.36% (95% CI 0.18– 0.61%). In trials with oral drugs, nocebo AEs and dropout frequencies were 19.82% (95% CI 15.84–24.12%) and 0.33% (95% CI 0.17–0.55%). Forty-five trials for the migraine prevention were analyzed. Nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout frequencies were estimated to be 42.78% (95% CI 34.73–51.36%) and 4.75% (95% CI 3.28– 6.45%). Nocebo frequencies between trials for botulin toxin type A (BoTA) and topiramate did not differ significantly except that nocebo dropouts in trials for BoTA were significantly lower than the average for all other prophylactic anti-migraine treatments (0.22% vs. 4.755%). In trials for TTH (only four trials were retrieved in this meta-analysis), nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout frequencies were 23.99% (95% CI 4.61–52.20%) and 5.44% (95% CI 1.32–12.12%). No sufficient data to analyze trials for symptomatic treatment of TTH were found by the authors, as for the preventive treatment of cluster headache. For symptomatic cluster headache treatment, four trials were analyzed in this meta-analysis, and the nocebo AE was estimated at 18.67% (95% CI 10.65–28.33%); insufficient data were gathered to calculate the nocebo dropouts (Table 7.1) [26]. 78 C. Deligianni and D. D. Mitsikostas Table 7.1 Nocebo in placebo randomized controlled studies for primary headache treatment Condition Migraine, symptomatic Migraine, prevention TTH, symptomatic TTH, prevention Cluster headache, symptomatic Cluster headache, prevention % Nocebo AEs (CI) 18.4 (14.9–22.2) 42.8 (34.7–51.4) NA 24.0 (4.6–52.2) NA 18.7 (1.6–28.3) % Nocebo dropouts (CI) 0.3 (0.2–0.5) 4.7 (3.3–6.5) NA 5.4 (1.3–12.1) NA NA Nocebo AEs patients treated with placebo and experienced an adverse event, nocebo dropouts patients treated with placebo and discontinued treatment because of adverse event; CI confidence intervals, NA non-applicable, TTH tension-type headache [26] Table 7.2 The Q-No questionnaire Question I read the summary of product characteristics (SPC) before taking a medication I have discontinued a medication because of adverse effects in the past I ask my physician for potential adverse effects of the medication he/she gives me I take into account the adverse effects reported in the summary of product characteristics (SPC) seriously Total score Rating Rating: 1 = never, 5 = always; by using a cutoff at score 15, the Q-No predicts nocebo with 71.7% specificity, 67.5% sensitivity, and 42.5% positive predictive value [23] 7.4 Q-No Questionnaire To capture patients with potential future nocebo responses, a specific self-fulfilled questionnaire (Q-No) was evaluated with 71.7% specificity, 67.5% sensitivity, and 42.5% positive predictive value [23]. Q-No is a four-item (rating range 4–20) questionnaire addressing issues related to nocebo in outpatients seeking neurological consultation. When the total score is higher than 15 (Table 7.2), the physician should educate the patient for nocebo to minimize the potential patients’ negative expectations. Because several unpredictable factors may influence patients’ expectations during medical treatment, it is difficult to guess nocebo; thus this tool may help physicians to uncover those patients and treat them appropriately. There are several limitations of the Q-No evaluation, however. Specificity, positive predictive value, and reliability are relatively low. Because participants in the evaluation of Q-No have had discussed the nocebo phenomenon with the treating neurologists, they were partially educated; thus nocebo responses may differ in this patients’ sample comparing to naive patients. Besides these limitations, however, Q-No may serve as a useful tool to predict potential nocebo responses in clinical practice. Capturing these patients and educating and treating them closely may limit nocebo and its obvious and severe consequences in outcome [23]. 7 Nocebo in Headache Treatment Table 7.3 Nocebo adverse events in outpatients suffering from primary headache disorders 79 Headache disorder Migraine Episodic migraine Chronic migraine Tension-type headache Episodic TTH Chronic TTH Cluster headache Episodic cluster headache Chronic cluster headache Patients scored ≥15 in Q-No (%) 217/372 (58.3) 142/220 (64.5) 75/152 (49.3) 55/107 (51.4) 16/25 (64.0) 39/82 (47.6) 19/35 (54.3) 9/14 (64.3) 10/21 (47.6) Ref. [24] All 58,3 64,3 Migraine Episodic Chronic 49,3 All 51,4 TTH Episodic 64 Chronic 47,6 All 54,3 Cluster Headache Episodic 64,3 Chronic 47,6 0 17,5 35 52,5 70 patients (%) scored ≥ 15 in Q-No Fig. 7.1 Nocebo adverse events (%) in outpatients suffering from primary headache disorders. Patients suffering from episodic forms showed increased prevalence for potential behaviors than those suffering from chronic forms in all three primary headache disorders (score ≥ 15 in Q-No). ΤΤΗ tension-type headache [24] 7.5 Nocebos in Headache Outpatient Sufferers In a clinical observational study [24], 514 outpatients suffering from several headache disorders fulfilled the Q-No, and 291 (56.6%) scored more than 15 indicating potential nocebo behaviors (Table 7.3 and Fig. 7.1). Those who were suffering from episodic forms of all three primary headache disorders (migraine, TTH, and cluster headache) displayed higher risk for nocebo behaviors compared to those who were suffering from chronic forms: two out of three patients suffering from episodic headaches scored more than 15 in Q-No compared to one out of two patients who are suffering from chronic subtypes. Migraineurs also showed higher risk for nocebo 80 C. Deligianni and D. D. Mitsikostas than those who were suffering from TTH (58.3% vs. 51.4%) [24]. In addition, nocebo influenced patients’ choices for the treatment. These data indicate that nocebo effect may worsen pain outcomes in essential proportion of headache outpatients, raising the need for better understanding the origin and the factors controlling nocebo. 7.6 Factors Influencing Nocebos in Headaches Multiple factors, both internal and environmental, encompassed within a clinical encounter create a context through which patients develop negative or positive expectancies about treatments and clinical outcomes. Nocebo arises from negative expectancies elicited through verbal suggestion, conditioning, and/or social observation [3]. Like in other pain conditions, nocebo in headache has the potential to significantly influence pain pathways by triggering physiological changes that could consequently affect not only pain perception but also pharmacological efficacy and clinical outcomes in the context of pain management [8]. Human studies showed that nocebo effects can be effectively minimized by positive expectation induction and can even turn into placebo effects [2]. In Table 7.4 factors controlling nocebo are listed. It has been shown that pretrial verbal suggestion directly changes the patients’ expectations [4]; thus close follow-up and patient education may limit nocebo [21]. These influences and many others unpredictable like social media or the Internet information cannot be detected. Instead an individual’s negative expectation originating from internal psychological structures could be predicted. A previous experience of drug-related AEs and/or failed interventions are stable factors contributing to nocebo [9, 21, 22]. Whether these previously experienced AEs were or not related to nocebo may not change the risk for future nocebo responses. However, a negative patient–doctor communication facilitates nocebo [12]. Additionally, affective and cognitive traits could be important as well. Some personality traits and psychological factors, such as anxiety, harm avoidance and persistence, pessimism, and fear of Table 7.4 Factors influencing nocebo Modifiable Patient’s expectations Patient’s education Pre-treatment verbal suggestions Speed of treatment titration Safety profile of treatment Patient–doctor relation/communication Close follow-up Affective and cognitive traits The appearance of medical devices Pharmacophobia Non-modifiable Previous negative experiences Personality Cultural factors Drug-related adverse events Social media and the Internet information Gender Treatment price Generic formulations Age 7 Nocebo in Headache Treatment 81 pain, may influence the responsiveness to nocebo [2]. Finally, there is data indicating that treatment price and gender may influence nocebo responses in human studies, women being linked to conditioning rather than to expectancy and men the opposite, but this finding needs to be confirmed clinically [17, 37]. By applying the Q-No in patients prior to consultation (e.g., in the waiting room), a physician treating headache patients can easily be informed for potential nocebo behaviors and load specific and tailored approaches to limit them (see below). 7.7 Management of Nocebos in Clinical Practice Not only physicians but also nurses, other health-care professionals, and allied health services should be aware of their responsibility to avoid and reduce nocebo effects and the detrimental consequences of these effects, from diagnosis to therapy to prognosis [6]. The benefit of taking a medication (“this drug will reduce your migraine days from 8 you have now to 3 per month”) should always be included in patient information about newly prescribed drugs. Alternatively, a physician may explain the treatment benefit by using only positive outcomes, like “this drug will increase your headachefree days from 22 to 27 per month.” Drug treatment for primary headache disorders can be started at a very low dose (half or one-quarter the recommended starting dose). The potential advantages of this approach include a further decrease in the risk of adverse events, enhanced patient participation, improved adherence, and reduced long-term costs [19]. “Start low, go slow” approach when starting a new medication also improves adherence in nocebo patients by minimizing the risk of AEs. Most importantly physicians should use an authentic and empathic communication style, providing adequate information regarding disease, diagnoses, treatments, and AEs. Close follow-up and discussing patients’ anxieties, concerns, and expectations are always required. Occasionally, it is very difficult to convince the patient, but it helps much when the physician explains the origin and the prevalence of nocebo. Informing the patient that he/she is not the only one who has this reaction (in headaches almost half of patients report nocebo AEs) and understanding the brain mechanisms underlying nocebo make the patient feel comfortable (Box 7.1) [6, 9, 12, 21, 22]. Box 7.1 Tips to Limit Nocebo Apply the Q-No questionnaire before consultation; if your patient scores ≥15, put on the following: • During the interview try to maximize placebo and minimize nocebo by being positive, authentic, and empathic; having face contact optimizes treatment expectation and expectation of adverse effects; ask for potential nocebo risk factors (e.g., previous experiences of drug-related AEs; use of complementary/ alternative medicine treatments); explain the origin, the brain mechanisms, and the prevalence of nocebo in general and in headaches in particular. 82 C. Deligianni and D. D. Mitsikostas • When prescribing a new medical treatment, select the ones with the better safety profile, instead of those with the better efficacy; use positive outcomes to explain their benefits (e.g., “this treatment will increase your headache-free days from 22 to 27 per month”); inform the patient only for the most frequent potential adverse events always in relation to the treatment benefit; ask the patient to participate in the treatment choice by presenting different options; “start low, go slow” with new medication treatment titration. • Close follow-up to monitor potential adverse events (ask for a telephone follow-up a month after). • Be tolerant and persistent, because nocebo patients require more attention and time to convince adhering than other patients do. 7.8 Conclusions Collectively, nocebo effects could substantially reduce treatment efficacy and tolerability, and therefore patients’ adherence and compliance, and could play a major role in their withdrawal from necessary treatment. On the other hand, placebo increases treatments’ efficacy firmly and can be triggered by positive verbal pretrial suggestions. Because both nocebo and placebo may represent two opposite pathways that coexist in humans, it is necessary to modify them, but one cannot remove them. The placebo phenomenon may promote appetitive and safety behaviors, while nocebo effects may favor perceptual mechanisms that are initiated to prevent dangerous events and negative outcomes [7]. In general, physicians treating headaches should acknowledge nocebo as a significant cofactor for treatment adherence and failure and plan techniques to border nocebo, such as patients’ education and close follow-up. Pragmatic but enthusiastic presentation of treatment options increases placebo and limits nocebo. Most headache conditions share a genetic component that no treatment can erase. On the other hand, some headache patients avoid preventative treatment because they believe their headaches are untreatable. To recruit those patients, a long interview is required to explain the treatment benefits over the potential risks. Even for the worst case, there is a treatment that could improve the patient’s life quality. The Q-No is a useful tool to capture other nocebo cases that need special management. References 1. Amanzio M, Corazzini LL, Vase L, Benedetti F. A systematic review of adverse events in placebo groups of anti-migraine clinical trials. Pain. 2009;146(3):261–9. 2. Bartels DJP, van Laarhoven AIM, Stroo M, Hijne K, Peerdeman KJ, Donders ART, van de Kerkhof PCM, Evers AWM. Minimizing nocebo effects by conditioning with verbal suggestion: a randomized clinical trial in healthy humans. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0182959. 7 Nocebo in Headache Treatment 83 3. Blassini M, Corsi N, Klinger R, Colloca L. Nocebo and pain: an overview of the psychoneurobiological mechanisms. Pain Rep. 2017;2(2). pii: e585. 4. Benedetti F, Amanzio M. The placebo response: how words and rituals change the patient’s brain. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;84(3):413–9. 5. Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When word are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience. 2007;147:260–71. 6. Bingel U. Avoiding nocebo effects to optimize treatment outcome. JAMA. 2014;312:693–4. 7. Colloca L. Placebo, nocebo, and learning mechanisms. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2014;225:17–35. 8. Colloca L, Grillon C. Understanding placebo and nocebo responses for pain management. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2014;18:419. 9. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA. 2012;307:567–8. 10. Dodd S, Schacht A, Kelin K, Dueñas H, Reed VA, Williams LJ, Quirk FH, Malhi GS, Berk M. Nocebo effects in the treatment of major depression: results from an individual study participant-level meta-analysis of the placebo arm of duloxetine clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76(6):702–11. 11. GBD 2016 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. 2017;390:1211–59. 12. Greville-Harris M, Dieppe P. Bad is more powerful than good: the nocebo response in medical consultations. Am J Med. 2015;128:126–9. 13. Gracia-Naya M, Santos-Lasaosa S, Ríos-Gómez C, Sánchez-Valiente S, García-Gomara MJ, Latorre-Jiménez AM, Artal-Roy J, Mauri-Llerda JA. Predisposing factors affecting dropout rates in preventive treatment in a series of patients with migraine. Rev Neurol. 2011;53: 201–8. 14. Gupta A, Thompson D, Whitehouse A, Collier T, Dahlof B, Poulter N, Collins R, Sever P, ASCOT Investigators. Adverse events associated with unblinded, but not with blinded, statin therapy in the Anglo-Scandinavian Cardiac Outcomes Trial-Lipid-Lowering Arm (ASCOTLLA): a randomized double blind placebo-controlled trial and its non-randomized non-blind extension phase. Lancet. 2017;389(10088):2473–81. 15. Hepp Z, Dodick DW, Varon SF, Gillard P, Hansen RN, Devine EB. Adherence to oral migraine-preventive medications among patients with chronic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2015;35: 478–88. 16. Hepp Z, Bloudek LM, Varon SF. Systematic review of migraine prophylaxis adherence and persistence. J Manag Care Pharm. 2014;20:22–33. 17. Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Braun S, Kowalski A, Schrauth M, Zipfel S, Enck P. Gender and the nocebo response following conditioning and expectancy. J Psychosom Res. 2009;66(4):323–8. 18. Loder E, Goldstein R, Biondi D. Placebo effects in oral triptan trials: the scientific and ethical rationale for continued use of placebo controls. Cephalalgia. 2005;25:124–31. 19. McCormack JP, Allan GM, Virani AS. Is bigger better? An argument for very low starting doses. CMAJ. 2011;183:65–9. 20. Meister R, Jansen A, Härter M, Nestoriuc Y, Kriston L. Placebo and nocebo reactions in randomized trials of pharmacological treatments for persistent depressive disorder. A metaregression analysis. J Affect Disord. 2017;215:288–98. 21. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headaches: implications for clinical practice and trial design. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2012;12:132–7. 22. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headache. Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;29(3):331–6. 23. Mitsikostas DD, Deligianni CI. Q-No: a questionnaire to predict nocebo in & outpatients seeking neurological consultation. Neurol Sci. 2015;36:379–81. 24. Mitsikostas DD, Belesioti I, Arvaniti C, Mitropoulou E, Deligianni C, Kasioti E, Constantinidis T, Dermitzakis M, Vikelis M. Hellenic Headache Society. Patients’ preferences for headache acute and preventive treatment. J Headache Pain. 2017;18(1):102. 84 C. Deligianni and D. D. Mitsikostas 25. Mitsikostas DD, Chalarakis NG, Mantonakis LI, Delicha EM, Sfikakis PP. Nocebo in fibromyalgia: meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials and implications for practice. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(5):672–80. 26. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis LI, Chalarakis NG. Nocebo is the enemy, not placebo. A meta-analysis of reported side effects after placebo treatment in headaches. Cephalalgia. 2011;31:550–61. 27. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis L, Chalarakis N. Nocebo in clinical trials for depression: a metaanalysis. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(1):82–6. 28. Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Colloca L, Amanzio M, Price DD, Jensen TS, Vase L. The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis. Pain. 2014;155(8):1426–34. 29. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. A meta-analytic approach to estimating nocebo effects in neuropathic pain trials. J Neurol. 2012;259:436–47. 30. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effects in multiple sclerosis trials: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler. 2010;16:816–28. 31. Reuter U, Sanchez del Rio M, Carpay JA, Boes CJ, Silberstein SD. GSK headache masters program: placebo adverse events in headache trials: headache as an adverse event of placebo. Cephalalgia. 2003;23:496–503. 32. Rozen TD, Fishman RS. Cluster headache in the United States of America: demographics, clinical characteristics, triggers, suicidality, and personal burden. Headache. 2012;52:99–113. 33. Silva MA, Duarte GS, Camara R, Rodrigues FB, Fernandes RM, Abreu D, Mestre T, Costa J, Trenkwalder C, Ferreira JJ. Placebo and nocebo responses in restless legs syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2017;88(23):2216–24. 34. Silvestri A, Galetta P, Cerquetani E, Marazzi G, Patrizi R, Fini M, Rosano GM. Report of erectile dysfunction after therapy with beta-blockers is related to patient knowledge of side effects and is reversed by placebo. Eur Heart J. 2003;24(21):1928–32. 35. Stathis P, Smpiliris M, Konitsiotis S, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo as a potential confounding factor in clinical trials for Parkinson’s disease treatment: a meta-analysis. Eur J Neurol. 2013;20:527–33. 36. Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Katsarava Z, Lainez JM, Lampl C, Lantéri-Minet M, Rastenyte D, Ruiz de la Torre E, Tassorelli C, Barré J, Andrée C. The impact of headache in Europe: principal results of the Eurolight project. J Headache Pain. 2014;15:31. 37. Tinnermann A, Geuter S, Sprenger C, Finsterbusch J, Büchel C. Interactions between brain and spinal cord mediate value effects in nocebo hyperalgesia. Science. 2017;358(6359):105–8. 38. Webster RK, Weinman J, Rubin GJ. A systematic review of factors that contribute to nocebo effects. Health Psychol. 2016;35(12):1334–55. 39. Zaccara G, Giovannelli F, Giorgi FS, Franco V, Gasparini S. Analysis of nocebo effects of antiepileptic drugs across different conditions. J Neurol. 2016;263(7):1274–9. 40. Zis P, Shafiq F, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effect in refractory partial epilepsy during pre-surgical monitoring: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. Seizure. 2017;45:95–9. Chapter 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents Vanda Faria and David Borsook 8.1 Introduction The role of placebo and nocebo in pediatrics has received relatively little attention compared with adults [35, 74, 86]. Migraine, known to have a high placebo response rate, especially in children [29, 79], is an ideal disease state to study both the placebo and nocebo phenomena. While the specific pathophysiology of the disease is still unknown, migraine has a number of attributes that allow a deeper investigation of the underlying efficacy resultant from potential positive and motivational therapeutic interventions like placebo or potential harmful contributions like nocebo. First, its intermittent nature (i.e., in episodic migraine, migraine attacks are present for 14 or less days a month), with an apparent interictal period where patients are overtly “normal” with no headache, provides an ideal avenue for evaluating placebo and nocebo responses on sensory effects of the disease (i.e., headache) in patients during a pain-free state vs. healthy controls. In other words, it allows the assessment of the relative sensitivity of these patients to experimental manipulations such as placebo and nocebo. For instance, patients with migraine seem to be more responsive to aversive stimuli [89] suggesting that they should be more responsive to nocebo than healthy controls. Secondly, aside from the headache pain itself, there are associated sensory phenomena such as photophobia, phonophobia, and osmophobia that provide further evaluator processes for placebo and nocebo measures. Finally, episodic migraine provides a range of severity and frequency of attacks, i.e., from low to high, where the latter is considered to provide a brain state that is more or less resistant to placebo responses with greater brain changes (functional and structural) occurring in patients with the higher frequency of migraine. V. Faria (*) · D. Borsook Center for Pain and the Brain, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA e-mail: [email protected]; [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_8 85 86 V. Faria and D. Borsook Expectations of clinical benefit or clinical worsening seem to be central for placebo and nocebo responses [43]. Previous pharmacological and neuroimaging studies have been focusing on understanding how expectancies interact with distinct biological systems to shape clinical responses [32]. However, the great majority of these studies have been focusing on the beneficial aspects of expectancies, and only recently, placebos counterpart, i.e., nocebos, started to receive wider attention from the scientific community [45]. From a clinical perspective, both placebo and nocebo effects play a central role into the clinical outcome. When it comes to migraine, whereas positive expectancies of migraine treatment may lead to reductions in migraine frequency and even complete migraine remission, negative expectations can reduce the effectiveness of migraine therapies and even produce side effects, usually consistent with the side effects observed with the actual migraine drug intake. Hence, whereas placebo responses, when effectively harnessed, can be one of the best allies of good clinical practice, nocebo responses are equally important in the optimization of the clinical outcome. Negative expectations may trigger anxiety resulting in a cascade of adverse symptoms or experiences. Consequently, these adverse experiences may strengthen the negative expectations, making the nocebo effect stronger [91]. It becomes imperative to advance and to translate both the placebo and the nocebo knowledge to improve clinical care, especially in pediatrics, where the use of alternative therapies, with no pharmacological long-term side effects, becomes even more appealing. Moreover, the pediatric group may also provide a “naïve” population in that their life experiences are more limited than adults and potentially more influenced by adult behavior [41, 81] (i.e., parental and clinician). We begin this chapter by providing a brief overview of pediatric migraine, its clinical characterization, and current treatments. We then introduce placebos and nocebos by reviewing placebo and nocebo effects in the field of pain with adults (its biological underpinnings), in pediatric clinical trials, and in the pediatric migraine context. Finally we provide a description of current insights into potential learning mechanisms underlying placebo and nocebo responses, its implications in the pediatric migraine clinic, and ethics of placebo administration in the pediatric setting (i.e., Placebo and Nocebo Effects in the Pediatric Clinic). Our focus is on how to best optimize treatment outcomes in pediatric migraine practice in an ethical way by maximizing the placebo and minimizing the nocebo effects during therapeutic interventions. 8.2 8.2.1 Pediatric Migraine linical Presentation of Migraine in Children C and Adolescents Headache is the most common manifestation of pain in childhood occurring in up to 75% of school-aged children [10]. Pediatric migraine is the most frequent recurrent headache disorder occurring in up to 28% of older children [3]. Frequently starting in childhood and extending into adulthood, migraine has been 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 87 identified as the seventh highest specific cause of adult disability worldwide [26] and is thought to be a common underdiagnosed cause behind recurrent headaches. The estimated overall (i.e., data from 32 countries) mean prevalence of pediatric headache is 54.4% (95% CI: 43.1–65.8) and 9.1% for pediatric migraine (95% CI: 7.1–11.1) [90]. Migraine is a complex neurological disorder affecting multiple cortical, subcortical, and brainstem areas regulating autonomic, affective, cognitive, and sensory functions [17]. Hence, an effort to unravel the pathophysiology of migraine must go beyond the simplistic view of “migraine generator” regions. The clinical presentation of pediatric migraine is comparable to that of adults, which is characterized by recurrent intermittent headaches (1–14 headache days per month in episodic migraine and >14 headache days in chronic migraine), lasting 4–72 h. However in pediatric migraine, the attacks tend to be of shorter duration (i.e., 30 min to 48 h) and bilateral. Besides, in children, non-headache and neurological symptoms, characterized as migraine variants (i.e., gastrointestinal, autonomic, and non-nociceptive symptoms) [29, 40], may be more prominent than the actual headache. In addition, migraine is associated with a number of comorbidities such as asthma, allergies [1, 25, 52], sleep disorders [49], emotional and behavioral problems [78], depression, and anxiety [62]. Migraine is more common in boys than in girls until after the menarche, when it becomes more common in girls [77]. Neuroimaging studies have started to provide some insights into developmental migraine sexrelated susceptibility and/or resilience [31]. There is no formal classification of migraine specific to children, but the International Classification of Headache Disorders (ICHD) from the International Headache Society (IHS) distinguishes the variants of migraine in children (https://doi.org/10.1177/0333102413485658). About 65–80% of the pediatric population with migraine will experience disruption of their normal life at home, in school, and in other social settings. Hence, early recognition and successful management of the disorder becomes vital for the affected children. 8.2.2 Treatment of Migraine in Children and Adolescents An early diagnosis and a prompt therapy are imperative when it comes to the success of migraine treatment. The successful management of migraine involves recognizing the triggering factors, providing pain relief, and considering prophylaxis. It entails a multimodal or biopsychosocial approach combining abortive and prophylactic pharmacological therapies and non-pharmacological therapies such as behavioral and psychoeducational interventions (patient education and lifestyle strategies) [28]. Whereas the treatment short-term goals are focused on the symptoms (i.e., to relieve pain and nausea and to promote sleep), the longterm goals are focused on the patient’s quality of life (i.e., by reducing the frequency and severity of headaches) and on a long-term successful management of the disorder. 88 V. Faria and D. Borsook 8.2.2.1 Pharmacological Therapies When it comes to pharmacological interventions, most pediatric placebo-controlled trials have failed to demonstrate effectiveness of drugs over placebo, mostly due to the high placebo response rates [29, 79]. For instance, Powers et al. [66] recently reported that a pediatric placebo-controlled trial was stopped following an interim analysis showing no difference between amitriptyline and topiramate as compared to placebo. Only two triptans (almotriptan and rizatriptan) have been approved by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) to be safe and effective for the abortive treatment of pediatric migraine [55], and only one antiepileptic drug (topiramate) and one antidepressant (trazodone) have been shown to be more effective than placebo when it comes to prophylactic treatments [29]. 8.2.2.2 Non-pharmacological Therapies Regarding non-pharmacological interventions, behavioral techniques such as cognitive behavioral therapy and stress management therapy have been shown to be highly effective. In fact, stress management seems to provide a better clinical outcome than pharmacological intervention (i.e., the ß-blocker metoprolol) [71]. Complementary and alternative medicines such as acupuncture and homeopathic interventions have also been reported to be as effective as, or even more effective than, prophylactic pharmacological migraine treatments [24, 57]. Importantly, however, neither acupuncture nor homeopathic therapies seem to outperform placebos in clinical trials [22, 72] suggesting that the observable therapeutic responses might reflect the placebo effect, perhaps enhanced by different levels of elaborated rituals, and a closer patient-practitioner interaction. 8.3 8.3.1 Placebo and Nocebo Effects lacebo and Nocebo Analgesia: Insights from Adult P Studies Even though the neurobiological mechanisms behind placebo effects have been investigated for several conditions, analgesia is without a doubt the best-studied placebo response [85]. Pain is a highly complex subjective experience with a vast biological significance. The experience of pain is not linearly related to the nociceptive input; it contains both sensory and affective dimensions that are under the influence of several factors [61]. Hence, pain experiences are mediated not only by peripherally related bottom-up mechanisms but also via centrally related topdown processes. Psychosocial regulation of pain together with the possibility to induce pain experimentally makes pain an excellent model for assessing placebo 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 89 and nocebo responses. As for placebo effects, nocebo effects are best studied in the field of pain [27]. However, the nocebo effect has been widely overlooked, and only recently this equally important response started to gain visibility in the field. Due to ethical constraints, it is not easy to gather empirical data to best comprehend this phenomenon. However, it has been suggested that nocebo hyperalgesia can be considered as a stress response. Nocebo suggestions induce anxiety, which in turn results in pain enhancement [19, 91]. Notably however, all this knowledge comes from adult studies, and developmental differences between children and adults (e.g., neurological, cognitive, and psychological) that might significantly impact positive and negative analgesic responses in children haven’t been considered. 8.3.1.1 iological Constructs (Brain Regions) of Placebo Analgesia B in the Developing Brain Neuroimaging studies investigating the underlying neural mechanism of placebo analgesia have shown the repeated involvement of brain regions including the medial thalamus, anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, periaqueductal gray, secondary somatosensory cortex, dorsal posterior insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, ventral striatum, and rostroventral medulla [85]. Whereas some of the regions are related to higher functions such as attention, thought, and consciousness (e.g., prefrontal regions) and are thought to be involved in appraisal and in the generation of placebo-related expectancies (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), other regions are involved in sensory perception/pain intensity (e.g., S1/dorsal posterior insula), salience (e.g., anterior insula), reward-aversion/motivational state, etc. [85]. Notably, these regions have been evaluated in adults and thus portray the neurophysiology of placebo analgesia in a mature brain. However, within a developing brain, functional connectivity changes in childhood and adolescence [13, 73, 88]. With altered connectivity between different brain regions during development, the placebo response may be changed just as placebos can change the brain [4]. In addition, the specific evolution of a particular brain region during development, for example, the anterior cingulate, may alter the susceptibility to behavior [83]. Thus, the placebo response may differ with development in childhood and adolescence based on brain maturation. In fact, when it comes to expectancies and belief systems, children have the ability to approach a situation from an unbiased or naïve perspective. They seem motivated to learn, have fewer prejudices and less experience, and show a less developed ability for inhibition in general [18]. A mature prefrontal cortex, for instance, may hamper flexible thinking, due to prior experience that could affect and bias expectations. Children on the other hand have a stronger appetitive system and weaker control system. Delayed prefrontal maturation makes children’s belief system more flexible and easier to shape [44]; hence, they might be more open to follow suggestions from clinicians and/or parents as compared to adults. 90 8.3.2 V. Faria and D. Borsook Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Pediatric Clinical Trials The use of placebos in adult clinical trials has proven priceless in providing a baseline against which active new treatments are assessed. Until recently, most knowledge on pediatric pharmacological treatments has been based on evidence that originated from adult clinical trials [76]. This, however, constitutes a major challenge for clinicians that have to prescribe safe and effective doses of therapeutic agents in pediatric populations. Nevertheless, due to the increasing awareness that children are not mere small adults and that developmental factors impact the pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of drugs [69], FDA and NIH published, during the last decade of the past century, guidelines securing the inclusion of pediatric groups in the study of new compounds with potential use in children [87]. Consequently, there have been an increasing number of pediatric trials testing the superiority of active new drugs against placebos. However, as previously stated, most of these trials are facing the challenge of high placebo response rates [36, 67, 79]. The fact that clinical relevant symptom improvements are observed after placebo administration poses major methodological difficulties for pediatric trials, as significant differential outcomes between active interventions and placebo become harder to detect. As placebos, nocebos are seen as a significant burden in clinical trials. Nocebo effects can be so significant that patients eventually drop out of the study [45]. In the pediatric field, when comparing the frequency of adverse events between active treatments and placebo treatments in clinical trials of depression, no differences were found between both arms suggesting a considerable nocebo effect [68]. Importantly, as reported above, these adverse responses are commonly related not only with high rates of dropouts but also with difficulty in assessing the efficacy and the safety profile of drugs in clinical trials [45]. Negative expectations substantially limit treatment’s improvement, not only by increasing the magnitude of adverse events but also by decreasing the impact of improvement itself, or even by abolishing the beneficial drug effects [11, 33]. Regarding the experienced side effects, they are usually consistent with the effects experienced with the actual drug being tested, i.e., the adverse events expected by the patients and their physicians [50]. However, it is not always easy to disentangle nocebo effects from real drug side effects, but it is important to bear in mind that patients’ treatment expectations can have both positive (placebo) and negative consequences (nocebo) when evaluating the outcomes of the trials. 8.3.3 Pediatric Placebo Analgesia in Migraine The failure of pediatric pharmacological trials together with the success of nonpharmacological interventions highlights the importance of placebo in the management of headaches in children. In migraine trials, whereas placebo response rates 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 91 have been estimated to be around 35% in adults, pediatric trials have reported response rates around 50% or higher [54]. Placebo analgesia seems to be an even a greater burden for pediatric trials than for adult trials [79]. Studies suggest that placebo response rates are significantly more marked in children as compared to adults across conditions [86]. Age seems to play a role in placebo responsivity with higher placebo response rates among younger children [59]. There seems to be an inverse relationship between age and placebo response [46]. Something happens in the developmental process that makes placebo response decline and drug response increase. From a clinical perspective, especially in pediatrics, one cannot ignore the fact that migraine symptoms significantly improve after placebo therapy in more than half of the children [54]. Placebo pills have been reported to decrease the average occurrence of headaches to fewer than three a month from a starting point of nearly six a month [29]. Moreover, it has been suggested that placebo interventions result in 32% of pediatric complete relief [39]. As reported above, however, it is important to understand that placebos can also result in clinically negative effects. Hence, the focus should be in understanding the mechanism behind successful pediatric placebo interventions to maximize the placebo benefits and minimize the nocebo effects in the migraine clinic. 8.4 Placebo and Nocebo Effects in the Pediatric Clinic 8.4.1 ossible Mechanisms Underlying Placebo and Nocebo P Responses Different forms of learning seem to underlie both placebo and the nocebo responses. Verbally induced suggestions, conditioning, and modeling can be seen as vehicles through which either positive (placebo) or negative (nocebo) expectations are acquired. 8.4.1.1 Verbally Induced Suggestions Verbally induced suggestions can influence the clinical experience positively or negatively [2]. Regarding the experience of pain, when participants are directed toward the analgesic properties of a treatment, analgesia is perceived, but when participants are alerted to the hyperalgesic effects of a treatment, pain is perceived [21]. The power of verbally induced suggestions has also been investigated in children [92]. Moreover, verbally induced expectations of no improvement (i.e., expecting a placebo pill when an active drug is given) may significantly reduce or abolish the beneficial effect of the actual drug [33]. Hence, the same way that positive verbal suggestions produce beneficial effects, negative verbal suggestions may produce harmful effects or disrupt the therapeutical beneficial effects. 92 8.4.1.2 V. Faria and D. Borsook Conditioning With regard to conditioning, past experiences of pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatments create subsequent placebo and nocebo responses depending on the positive or negative effects of the treatment. For instance, in a pediatric study of ADHD, children responded well to half their regular medication dose but only when given together with a placebo. Hence, pairing a conditioned stimulus, i.e., placebo pill, with the active drug resulted in conditioned placebo responses that allowed children with ADHD to be treated effectively with a lower dose of medication [7]. Hence, placebo substitution may be a promising way to harness the placebo effect in the clinic. According to conditioning, placebo effects can be elicited on the basis of a planned sequence of drug and conditioned stimuli. Importantly, however, conditioned placebo effects may also mimic the drug side effects i.e., nocebo effect [15], so it becomes important to be aware of this phenomenon to emphasize the beneficial aspects of the treatment in order to improve treatment outcomes in the clinic. 8.4.1.3 Modeling Modeling, or observing, and interacting with others also plays an important role in the formation of both placebo and nocebo responses. Observing a beneficial treatment in another person elicits a stable placebo analgesia [20]. Importantly, the effect size of observationally induced placebo analgesic (live or video) responses is comparable to those induced via a conditioning schedule [48]. Notably, it seems that observation carries potential cues to induce expectations of benefit and activate specific mechanisms independently of the social interactions [48]. The information drawn from modeling may create a self-projection into the future outcome increasing the expectation of analgesia or hyperalgesia, since the effects of observation apply to nocebo effect as well [84]. Importantly, in modeling, responses are elicited without direct experience, and these are essential aspects in the optimization of learning in general but especially in children. 8.4.2 Implications for Pediatric Migraine Treatment As noted above particular elements present themselves to enhance the placebo or nocebo effects during every clinical encounter. In pediatric migraine clinic, where placebo responses are particularly high [54, 79], awareness of the potential benefits of successfully harnessing these effects can lead to the optimization of the context surrounding the patient, in order to maximize the placebo component and minimize the nocebo component which can be crucial in migraine therapeutic outcomes. Knowledge of placebo and nocebo mechanisms can best be applied in pediatric migraine practice to enhance the positive aspects and diminish the negative aspects 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 93 of the patient-provider interaction to provide the additional therapeutic benefit and to avoid unhelpful characteristics in order to optimize the treatment outcome. Parent-child interactions are also critical in maintaining a positive process [12, 14, 35], so it becomes vital to educate the parents about the potential benefit and harm of these effects. 8.4.2.1 Enhancing Effects in the Clinic Since migraine is often triggered and/or aggravated by external factors such as anxiety and stress [14], it becomes crucial, in the clinical setting, to recognize and target the child’s individual needs, beliefs, and surroundings. During the doctor-patient encounter, the physician and the therapy are seen as possible rewards with the potential to suppress the discomfort caused by migraine. Within the therapeutic encounter, the physician must recognize and take the most of its role as being an integral part of the cure. The doctor-patient interaction is crucial for increasing treatment compliance and facilitating learning of new healthy behaviors [6] that might play an important role in preventive migraine attacks. Enhancing placebo effects in the pediatric migraine clinic may be considered as optimizing the “ecosystem.” In a therapeutical encounter, the treatment interaction is rather complex, and nowadays we have not yet reached an optimal manner of providing treatments in an environment that may enhance both positive and motivational cognitive processes. The ecosystem may be considered in a number of domains that are interconnected including: (a) Clinic Design: Well-designed architecture can prove a compelling, exciting, and “special place” to be. This has been an approach in Maggie’s Centers where “uplifting buildings [[read ‘clinics’]] benefit both body and soul” (www.e-architect.co.uk). The idea that placebo processes may be influenced by the physical environment has not been fully evaluated and implemented in the clinic. Just as social and cultural backgrounds may provide a bio-genomic basis for placebo responsivity (see [9, 37, 80]), the design of a clinic—its level of serenity, beauty, interactivity, and comfortability—may all contribute to the “special nature” of the place of treatment intervention [16]. The issue is to transform the treatment environment into a place that may further enhance the subtle and not so subtle contributions to potentiate placebo responsivity. (b) Behavioral Sciences: The need to introduce more robust processes that encompass the science behind the process of altering the brain to produce positive thoughts. The complexities of adding behavioral processes such as utilization of non-noxious perception [38] may contribute to alterations of attention and expectation [30]. Thus targeting processes that involve specific brain regions (e.g., prefrontal—parietal or temporal pole regions) through specific processes (akin to exercise for motor strength or sensorimotor integration) (ref) or cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT [44, 64]) need to be further evaluated and developed. 94 V. Faria and D. Borsook (c) Virtual Reality: Virtual reality can help with many aspects of the migraine disease and help mitigate associated fears and enhance the overall clinical condition [60]. Live brain training with automated feedback using fNIRS/EEG systems may become commonplace in the clinic to train the brain. (d) Communication: The role of verbal and other forms of communication in the clinic has been termed the “silent healer” [5, 8]. It is well known that specific aspects of the doctor-patient communication influence patients’ well-being such as treatment satisfaction, adherence to treatment, coping with the disease, and ultimately quality of life and state of health [5, 8, 82]. To improve the state of health or the therapeutic outcome, physicians are advised to deliver positive, motivational, and understandable information, reaching both the child and the parents. When it comes to providing information about the possible side effects of the chosen therapy, physicians should focus on minimizing the negative expectancies, i.e., nocebo effects, and the emphasis should be on the reasons why the treatment was chosen. Involving the patient in the therapeutical choices (i.e., patient-centered care) increases patients’ self-confidence and empowers them, which in turn may result in a better treatment adherence and improved healthcare outcomes (see [53, 63]). Empathy has the potential to set in motion psychological mechanisms that optimize the placebo response. A strong doctorpatient interaction based on care and interest might double the therapeutic response [52]. (e) Ongoing Interactions Through Social Media: The placebo effect has been getting more effective in clinical trials. A suggested reason or this relates to improved access to information for the general public. Recent evaluation of social media (e.g., Facebook) has been purported to be a process that may enhance the placebo effect (https://www.lodestonelogic.com); however this would need to be performed in a more targeted manner through clinics or support groups. Placebo responses are integral elements of any therapeutic intervention, which validates the significance of contextual and psychological factors in the healing process. The placebo effect can be of service to physicians in many clinical situations; hence, it should not be denied its rightful place in the pediatric clinic. 8.4.2.2 Enhancing Effects with Parental Interactions Migraine may be a difficult condition that may produce changes in children’s behavior that may be negatively or positively enhanced by their parents. It may create a functionally disabling negative feedback process (“social nocebo”) [7, 84]. Parentchild interactions have profound effects on individual behavior including pain [65, 75]. In the pediatric clinic, the need for parental support for noninvasive interventions and minimal risk interventions is very important. Parents are intrinsically motivated in the advancement of pediatric care and may display an emotional response when they think that the treatment is beneficial even in the absence of any 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 95 therapeutical benefit. This psychological benefit to parents (i.e., relief from worry) is called placebo by proxy [42]. Importantly, placebo by proxy and the placebo effect may interact to create a positive or a negative outcome [42]. For example, if parents feel empowered and optimistic about the treatment, there may occur a change in the patient’s environment that can result in less stress and anxiety. Hence, placebo by proxy may elicit changes in the patient’s psychosocial context that mediate the placebo effect and optimize the therapeutic outcome. The reverse can also occur resulting in worsening of the environment, more anxiety and stress, and consequently poorer outcome. In fact, placebo by proxy may create a false sense that the patient is benefitting from the treatment resulting in the prevention of a more appropriate treatment or neglect of important symptomatology that indicates that the patient is getting worse [42]. Hence, since placebo by proxy can interact with the placebo effect, it can be used as a clinical tool. For that reason, clinicians should be aware of placebo by proxy when making therapeutic decisions and involve the parents in these decisions to boost the belief and enthusiasms for the therapy. Importantly clinicians should also consider placebo by proxy when evaluating treatment response especially when parents are the primary source of therapeutic feedback. 8.4.3 thics of True Placebo Administration E in the Pediatric Setting Critics claim that prescribing placebo involves deception and therefore violates the patient’s autonomy and informed consent. On the other hand, supporters argue that the placebo response might be one of the most effective treatments available for many chronic conditions, and that can be accomplished without deception [56]. In pediatric populations with migraine, where there is still limited evidence supporting the benefit of pharmacotherapies and the potential negative effects of continued therapy on brain development are still considered a “black box,” the option of placebo therapy, shown to have high response rates, might be considered the most ethical choice. Survey studies show that doctors are willing to take advantage of placebo responses and prescribe placebo therapies in clinical practice [47], perhaps even more in children. Children, however, require special considerations and parental consent. Importantly, parents seem to consider the use of placebos acceptable in pediatric care, but their acceptance seems to be affected by the doctors’ opinions about the therapeutic benefits, the conditions for placebo use, safety, purity of placebos, and transparency [34]. Recent adult and pediatric trials have shown that clinically relevant placebo responses can be obtained in a nondeceptive, transparent manner (i.e., open-label placebo) [50, 51, 70]. In fact, being transparent to children and parents about the administration of placebo, as an adjuvant therapy for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, not only yielded equivalent therapeutic benefits, while reducing the medication use, but also resulted in fewer side effects [70]. 96 V. Faria and D. Borsook When it comes to headaches, studies have shown that overuse of abortive headache medication may lead to chronic daily headaches [58]. Hence, as described above, by means of learning via classical conditioning, placebos (as conditioned stimuli) might be alternated with the abortive headache medication (i.e., placebo substitution) to reduce the intake of abortive medication in these children [70]. This partial reinforcement method has been shown to produce fewer side effects without affecting the success of the treatment. Modeling can also be used as a vehicle to induce placebo analgesic responses in children with migraine in an ethical way. For instance, attending meetings of support groups where children with migraine meet other children with migraine and share experiences of how successful the therapies worked for them. Although our knowledge is limited by the lack of studies aimed at investigating the placebo and nocebo effects in pediatric migraine, progress in field has the potential to provide clinicians with the tools to maximize placebo effects and minimize nocebo effects in the clinic in an ethical way, and both doctors and parents seem ready to move forward. 8.5 Conclusions Despite the adverse impact of migraines on pediatric health, the current demand of effective treatments, together with the remarkably high placebo analgesic responsivity in children, the translation and adaption of the placebo and nocebo knowledge into the pediatric clinic of migraine is still in its infancy [35]. Migraine seems to be an ideal disease model to evaluate pediatric placebo responses. Due to the underlying nature of expectancies of the disease (i.e., expecting for the next attack), environmental factors and psychological mechanisms may be crucial in controlling migraine attacks [14]. Moreover, the pediatric brain is more plastic in terms of development and perhaps more adaptive [41, 81] to placebo processes. Hence, disregarding expectancy-related tools during the therapeutic encounter can be regarded as suboptimal care. Whereas positive expectations and beliefs lead to motivation and the adoption of assertive beneficial therapeutical responses, negative expectancies leads to inhibition and augmented anxiety and stress that will result in poorer therapeutical responses. By emphasizing the positive aspects of the treatment and providing continuous support, doctors and parents will not only increase the likelihood of maximizing placebo responses but also decrease the likelihood of nocebo responses [23]. Furthermore, there is some evidence that in adults migraine enhances or sensitizes the brain to aversive stimuli in the interictal period, suggesting that enhancing positive processes and removing negative ones should be a clear objective in the pediatric clinic [89]. Notably however, most of this knowledge comes from adult studies, and developmental differences between children and adults (e.g., neurological, cognitive, and psychological) that might significantly impact pediatric positive and negative analgesic responses haven’t been considered. Hence, future empirical work should care- 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 97 fully address these responses in the pediatric clinic to help physicians and parents to optimize the therapeutical outcome and to provide these children with an additional placebo benefit. References 1. Aamodt AH, Stovner LJ, Langhammer A, Hagen K, Zwart JA. Is headache related to asthma, hay fever, and chronic bronchitis? The Head-HUNT Study. Headache. 2007;47(2):204–12. 2. Annoni M, Miller FG. Placebo effects and the ethics of therapeutic communication: a pragmatic perspective. Kennedy Inst Ethics J. 2016;26(1):79–103. 3. Antonaci F, Voiticovschi-Iosob C, Di Stefano AL, Galli F, Ozge A, Balottin U. The evolution of headache from childhood to adulthood: a review of the literature. J Headache Pain. 2014;15:15. 4. Benedetti F, Carlino E, Pollo A. How placebos change the patient’s brain. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):339–54. 5. Benedetti F. How the doctor’s words affect the patient’s brain. Eval Health Prof. 2002;25(4):369–86. 6. Benedetti F. Placebo and the new physiology of the doctor-patient relationship. Physiol Rev. 2013;93(3):1207–46. 7. Benedetti F. Responding to nocebos through observation: social contagion of negative emotions. Pain. 2013;154(8):1165. 8. Bensing JM, Verheul W. The silent healer: the role of communication in placebo effects. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;80(3):293–9. 9. Bhugra D, Ventriglio A, Till A, Malhi G. Colour, culture and placebo response. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2015;61(6):615–7. 10. Bille B. Migraine and tension-type headache in children and adolescents. Cephalalgia. 1996;16(2):78. 11. Bingel U, Wanigasekera V, Wiech K, Ni Mhuircheartaigh R, Lee MC, Ploner M, et al. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci Transl Med. 2011;3(70):70ra14. 12. Birnie KA, Chambers CT, Chorney J, Fernandez CV, McGrath PJ. Dyadic analysis of child and parent trait and state pain catastrophizing in the process of children’s pain communication. Pain. 2016;157(4):938–48. 13. Blankenship SL, Redcay E, Dougherty LR, Riggins T. Development of hippocampal functional connectivity during childhood. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38(1):182–201. 14. Borsook D, Maleki N, Becerra L, McEwen B. Understanding migraine through the lens of maladaptive stress responses: a model disease of allostatic load. Neuron. 2012;73(2):219–34. 15. Brascher AK, Kleinbohl D, Holzl R, Becker S. Differential classical conditioning of the nocebo effect: increasing heat-pain perception without verbal suggestions. Front Psychol. 2017;8:2163. 16. Brown KK, Gallant D. Impacting patient outcomes through design: acuity adaptable care/ universal room design. Crit Care Nurs Q. 2006;29(4):326–41. 17. Burstein R, Noseda R, Borsook D. Migraine: multiple processes, complex pathophysiology. J Neurosci. 2015;35(17):6619–29. 18. Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(3):104–10. 19. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2007;20(5):435–9. 20. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain. 2009;144(1–2):28–34. 98 V. Faria and D. Borsook 21. Colloca L, Grillon C. Understanding placebo and nocebo responses for pain management. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2014;18(6):419. 22. Colquhoun D, Novella SP. Acupuncture is theatrical placebo. Anesth Analg. 2013;116(6):1360–3. 23. Czerniak E, Biegon A, Ziv A, Karnieli-Miller O, Weiser M, Alon U, Citron A. Manipulating the Placebo response in experimental pain by altering doctor’s performance style. Front Psychol. 2016;7:874. 24. Danno K, Colas A, Masson JL, Bordet MF. Homeopathic treatment of migraine in children: results of a prospective, multicenter, observational study. J Altern Complement Med. 2013;19(2):119–23. 25. Davey G, Sedgwick P, Maier W, Visick G, Strachan DP, HR A. Association between migraine and asthma: matched case-control study. Br J Gen Pract. 2002;52(482):723–7. 26. Disease GBD, Injury I, Prevalence C. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 diseases and injuries, 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. Lancet. 2016;388(10053):1545–602. 27. Dodd S, Dean OM, Vian J, Berk M. A review of the theoretical and biological understanding of the nocebo and placebo phenomena. Clin Ther. 2017;39(3):469–76. 28. Eidlitz-Markus T, Haimi-Cohen Y, Steier D, Zeharia A. Effectiveness of nonpharmacologic treatment for migraine in young children. Headache. 2010;50(2):219–23. 29. El-Chammas K, Keyes J, Thompson N, Vijayakumar J, Becher D, Jackson JL. Pharmacologic treatment of pediatric headaches: a meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(3):250–8. 30. Ernst MM, O’Brien HL, Powers SW. Cognitive-behavioral therapy: how medical providers can increase patient and family openness and access to evidence-based multimodal therapy for pediatric migraine. Headache. 2015;55(10):1382–96. 31. Faria V, Erpelding N, Lebel A, Johnson A, Wolff R, Fair D, Burstein R, Becerra L, Borsook D. The migraine brain in transition: girls vs boys. Pain. 2015;156(11):2212–21. 32. Faria V, Fredrikson M, Furmark T. Imaging the placebo response: a neurofunctional review. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18(7):473–85. 33. Faria V, Gingnell M, Hoppe JM, Hjorth O, Alaie I, Frick A, Hultberg S, Wahlstedt K, Engman J, Månsson KNT, Carlbring P, Andersson G, Reis M, Larsson EM, Fredrikson M, Furmark T. Do you believe it? Verbal suggestions influence the clinical and neural effects of escitalopram in social anxiety disorder: a randomized trial. EBioMedicine. 2017;24:179–88. 34. Faria V, Kossowsky J, Petkov MP, Kaptchuk TJ, Kirsch I, Lebel A, Borsook D. Parental attitudes about placebo use in children. J Pediatr. 2017;181:272–8.e10. 35. Faria V, Linnman C, Lebel A, Borsook D. Harnessing the placebo effect in pediatric migraine clinic. J Pediatr. 2014;165(4):659–65. 36. Fernandes R, Ferreira JJ, Sampaio C. The placebo response in studies of acute migraine. J Pediatr. 2008;152(4):527–33, 33.e1. 37. Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, Benedetti F. Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet. 2010;375(9715):686–95. 38. Fiorio M, Recchia S, Corra F, Simonetto S, Garcia-Larrea L, Tinazzi M. Enhancing non-noxious perception: behavioural and neurophysiological correlates of a placebo-like manipulation. Neuroscience. 2012;217:96–104. 39. Francis M. Placebo spectacles-an excellent non pharmacological treatment for pediatric migraine. Cephalalgia. 2013;33:8–255. 40. Galli F, D’Antuono G, Tarantino S, Viviano F, Borrelli O, Chirumbolo A, et al. Headache and recurrent abdominal pain: a controlled study by the means of the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL). Cephalalgia. 2007;27(3):211–9. 41. German TP, Defeyter MA. Immunity to functional fixedness in young children. Psychon Bull Rev. 2000;7(4):707–12. 42. Grelotti DJ, Kaptchuk TJ. Placebo by proxy. BMJ. 2011;343:d4345. 43. Hansen E, Zech N, Meissner K. [Placebo and nocebo: how can they be used or avoided?]. Internist (Berl). 2017;58(10):1102–10. 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 99 44. Harris P, Loveman E, Clegg A, Easton S, Berry N. Systematic review of cognitive behavioural therapy for the management of headaches and migraines in adults. Br J Pain. 2015;9(4): 213–24. 45. Hauser W, Hansen E, Enck P. Nocebo phenomena in medicine: their relevance in everyday clinical practice. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2012;109(26):459–65. 46. Ho TW, Fan X, Rodgers A, Lines CR, Winner P, Shapiro RE. Age effects on placebo response rates in clinical trials of acute agents for migraine: pooled analysis of rizatriptan trials in adults. Cephalalgia. 2009;29(7):711–8. 47. Howick J, Bishop FL, Heneghan C, Wolstenholme J, Stevens S, Hobbs FD, Lewith G. Placebo use in the United kingdom: results from a national survey of primary care practitioners. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58247. 48. Hunter T, Siess F, Colloca L. Socially induced placebo analgesia: a comparison of a prerecorded versus live face-to-face observation. Eur J Pain. 2014;18(7):914–22. 49. Isik U, Ersu RH, Ay P, Save D, Arman AR, Karakoc F, Dagli E. Prevalence of headache and its association with sleep disorders in children. Pediatr Neurol. 2007;36(3):146–51. 50. Kam-Hansen S, Jakubowski M, Kelley JM, Kirsch I, Hoaglin DC, Kaptchuk TJ, Burstein R. Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(218):218ra5. 51. Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, Sanchez MN, Kokkotou E, Singer JP, Kowalczykowski M, Miller FG, Kirsch I, Lembo AJ. Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e15591. 52. Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, Davis RB, Kerr CE, Jacobson EE, et al. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336(7651):999–1003. 53. Levinson W, Lesser CS, Epstein RM. Developing physician communication skills for patientcentered care. Health Aff (Millwood). 2010;29(7):1310–8. 54. Lewis DW, Winner P, Wasiewski W. The placebo responder rate in children and adolescents. Headache. 2005;45(3):232–9. 55. Lewis DW. Almotriptan for the acute treatment of adolescent migraine. Expert Opin Pharmacother. 2010;11(14):2431–6. 56. Lichtenberg P, Heresco-Levy U, Nitzan U. The ethics of the placebo in clinical practice. J Med Ethics. 2004;30(6):551–4. 57. Linde K, Allais G, Brinkhaus B, Manheimer E, Vickers A, White AR. Acupuncture for migraine prophylaxis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009;(1):CD001218. 58. Lucas S. Initial abortive treatments for migraine headache. Curr Treat Options Neurol. 2002;4(5):343–50. 59. Maas HJ, Danhof M, Della Pasqua OE. Analysis of the relationship between age and treatment response in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2009;29(7):772–80. 60. Malloy KM, Milling LS. The effectiveness of virtual reality distraction for pain reduction: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(8):1011–8. 61. Melzack R. From the gate to the neuromatrix. Pain. 1999;(Suppl 6):S121–6. 62. Merikangas KR, Angst J, Isler H. Migraine and psychopathology. Results of the Zurich cohort study of young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47(9):849–53. 63. Neumann M, Edelhauser F, Kreps GL, Scheffer C, Lutz G, Tauschel D, et al. Can patientprovider interaction increase the effectiveness of medical treatment or even substitute it?--an exploration on why and how to study the specific effect of the provider. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;80(3):307–14. 64. Ng QX, Venkatanarayanan N, Kumar L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for the management of pediatric migraine. Headache. 2017;57(3):349–62. 65. Oppenheim D. Child, parent, and parent-child emotion narratives: implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18(3):771–90. 100 V. Faria and D. Borsook 66. Powers SW, Coffey CS, Chamberlin LA, Ecklund DJ, Klingner EA, Yankey JW, et al. Trial of amitriptyline, topiramate, and placebo for pediatric migraine. N Engl J Med. 2017;376(2):115–24. 67. Rheims S, Cucherat M, Arzimanoglou A, Ryvlin P. Greater response to placebo in children than in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis in drug-resistant partial epilepsy. PLoS Med. 2008;5(8):e166. 68. Rojas-Mirquez JC, Rodriguez-Zuniga MJ, Bonilla-Escobar FJ, Garcia-Perdomo HA, Petkov M, Becerra L, et al. Nocebo effect in randomized clinical trials of antidepressants in children and adolescents: systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:375. 69. Samiee-Zafarghandy S, Mazer-Amirshahi M, van den Anker JN. Trends in paediatric clinical pharmacology data in US pharmaceutical labelling. Arch Dis Child. 2014;99(9):862–5. 70. Sandler AD, Glesne CE, Bodfish JW. Conditioned placebo dose reduction: a new treatment in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(5):369–75. 71. Sartory G, Muller B, Metsch J, Pothmann R. A comparison of psychological and pharmacological treatment of pediatric migraine. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36(12):1155–70. 72. Shang A, Huwiler-Muntener K, Nartey L, Juni P, Dorig S, Sterne JA, et al. Are the clinical effects of homoeopathy placebo effects? Comparative study of placebo-controlled trials of homoeopathy and allopathy. Lancet. 2005;366(9487):726–32. 73. Sherman LE, Rudie JD, Pfeifer JH, Masten CL, McNealy K, Dapretto M. Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;10:148–59. 74. Simmons K, Ortiz R, Kossowsky J, Krummenacher P, Grillon C, Pine D, et al. Pain and placebo in pediatrics: a comprehensive review of laboratory and clinical findings. Pain. 2014;155(11):2229–35. 75. Simons LE, Goubert L, Vervoort T, Borsook D. Circles of engagement: childhood pain and parent brain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:537–46. 76. Smith PB, Benjamin DK Jr, Murphy MD, Johann-Liang R, Iyasu S, Gould B, Califf RM, Li JS, Rodriguez W. Safety monitoring of drugs receiving pediatric marketing exclusivity. Pediatrics. 2008;122(3):e628–33. 77. Stewart WF, Wood C, Reed ML, Roy J, Lipton RB, Group AA. Cumulative lifetime migraine incidence in women and men. Cephalalgia. 2008;28(11):1170–8. 78. Strine TW, Okoro CA, McGuire LC, Balluz LS. The associations among childhood headaches, emotional and behavioral difficulties, and health care use. Pediatrics. 2006;117(5):1728–35. 79. Sun H, Bastings E, Temeck J, Smith PB, Men A, Tandon V, et al. Migraine therapeutics in adolescents: a systematic analysis and historic perspectives of triptan trials in adolescents. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(3):243–9. 80. Thompson JJ, Ritenbaugh C, Nichter M. Reconsidering the placebo response from a broad anthropological perspective. Cult Med Psychiatry. 2009;33(1):112–52. 81. Thompson-Schill SL, Ramscar M, Chrysikou EG. Cognition without control: when a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(5):259–63. 82. Verheul W, Sanders A, Bensing J. The effects of physicians’ affect-oriented communication style and raising expectations on analogue patients’ anxiety, affect and expectancies. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;80(3):300–6. 83. Vijayakumar N, Whittle S, Dennison M, Yucel M, Simmons J, Allen NB. Development of temperamental effortful control mediates the relationship between maturation of the prefrontal cortex and psychopathology during adolescence: a 4-year longitudinal study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;9:30–43. 84. Vogtle E, Barke A, Kroner-Herwig B. Nocebo hyperalgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain. 2013;154(8):1427–33. 85. Wager TD, Atlas LY. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(7):403–18. 86. Weimer K, Gulewitsch MD, Schlarb AA, Schwille-Kiuntke J, Klosterhalfen S, Enck P. Placebo effects in children: a review. Pediatr Res. 2013;74(1):96–102. 8 Placebos and Nocebos in Migraine: Children and Adolescents 101 87. Weisz G, Cambrosio A, Keating P, Knaapen L, Schlich T, Tournay VJ. The emergence of clinical practice guidelines. Milbank Q. 2007;85(4):691–727. 88. Wendelken C, Ferrer E, Ghetti S, Bailey SK, Cutting L, Bunge SA. Frontoparietal structural connectivity in childhood predicts development of functional connectivity and reasoning ability: a large-scale longitudinal investigation. J Neurosci. 2017;37(35):8549–58. 89. Wilcox SL, Veggeberg R, Lemme J, Hodkinson DJ, Scrivani S, Burstein R, et al. Increased functional activation of limbic brain regions during negative emotional processing in migraine. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:366. 90. Wober-Bingol C. Epidemiology of migraine and headache in children and adolescents. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2013;17(6):341. 91. Woo KY. Unravelling nocebo effect: the mediating effect of anxiety between anticipation and pain at wound dressing change. J Clin Nurs. 2015;24(13–14):1975–84. 92. Yujiro I, Shunji N. A psychosomatic study of contagious dermatitis. Kyushu J Med Sci. 1962;13:335–50. Chapter 9 Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders Panagiotis Zis 9.1 Introduction The term placebo refers to any intervention with no therapeutic effect that is used as a control in randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Treatment with placebo can produce a beneficial effect that cannot be attributed to the properties of the placebo itself (since it is inactive) and must, therefore, be due to the patient’s belief about treatment. Respectively, nocebo refers to the phenomenon of reporting/observing adverse events (AEs) when using a substance or treatment with no active therapeutic effect (placebo). Nocebo is probably the result of negative expectations by patients that medical treatment will probably harm rather than heal [17, 18]. Nocebo is associated with lower adherence to the therapeutic intervention, higher rates of treatment withdrawal, as well as significant difficulty in assessing the efficacy and the safety profile of a drug [1, 7]. Often, previous negative treatment experiences [2] along with several psychological factors such as stress and anxiety [6, 9, 14] control nocebo. Nocebo has been studied and found to be very prevalent in various neurological conditions, including epilepsy [33], motor neuron disease [24], multiple sclerosis [20], headache [15, 16], neuropathic pain [21], fibromyalgia [17], diabetic peripheral neuropathy [9], Meniere’s disease [4], restless legs syndrome [25], Parkinson’s disease [26], depression [18, 22], and Alzheimer’s disease [32]. This indicates significant implications for clinical practice related to treatment adherence and outcomes. In the present chapter, placebo and nocebo responses of non-traumatic brain disorders, other than headaches, are discussed. Only systematic previous and P. Zis (*) Academic Department of Neurosciences, Sheffield Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Sheffield, UK © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_9 103 104 P. Zis Table 9.1 Classification of the non-traumatic brain disorders based on the ICD-10 Classification Neoplastic ICD-10 chapter II ICD-10 block(s) C71 Psychiatric V F00-F99 Infectious Degenerative VI G00-G09 G20-G32 Demyelinating Episodic and paroxysmal G35-37 G40-47 Systemic atrophies affecting the brain G10-G13 Vascular I I61-I63 Example(s) Malignant neoplasm of the brain Schizophrenia (F20) Depressive episode (F32) Bacterial meningitis (G00) Parkinson’s disease (G20) Alzheimer’s disease (G30) Multiple sclerosis (G35) Epilepsy (G40) Migraine (G43) Hereditary ataxia (G11) Motor neuron disease (G12.2) Intracerebral hemorrhage (I61) Cerebral infarction (I63) ­ eta-analyses in the topic have been considered for the purposes of this chapter, as m such methodologies can provide the highest quality of evidence [3]. 9.2 Classification of Brain Disorders The non-traumatic brain disorders are broadly classified in demyelinating, degenerative, episodic, paroxysmal, infectious, neoplastic, vascular, systematic atrophies affecting the brain and psychiatric [30]. Table 9.1 summarizes this classification. Apart from headaches, published systematic reviews and meta-analyses to date are available in depression, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, and motor neuron disease. 9.3 9.3.1 ynopsis of Nocebo Phenomena in Non-traumatic Brain S Disorders Depression In a meta-analysis of 21 RCTs of various antidepressants, conducted by Mitsikostas et al. [18], the nocebo AE rate in depression was estimated to be 44.7%. The nocebo dropout rate was estimated to be 4.5%. In that study, the difference between the 9 Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders 105 nocebo rates and the respective rates of the active drug arms (active drug AE rate 40.9% and active drug dropout rate 6.9%) was very narrow. The univariate analysis identified three factors significantly correlating with the dropout rate because of AEs, age, placebo response, and duration of depression; however multivariate analysis failed to identify any particular factor contributing to nocebo. In a smaller meta-analysis [5] only of duloxetine RCTs, the nocebo AE rate was estimated to be higher (63.9%), whereas the nocebo dropout rate was very similar (4.7%). In that study, there was no evidence to support that reported AEs are influenced by the AEs mentioned in the clinical trial participant information and consent forms or that reported AEs would be influenced by AE profiles of previous antidepressant medications used by these study participants. However, people who had previously used complementary medications were more likely to report AEs [5]. 9.3.2 Alzheimer’s Disease In a meta-analysis, which included 20 RCTs and was conducted by Zis et al. [32], the nocebo AE rate in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) was estimated to be 57.8%. The nocebo dropout rate was estimated to be 6.6%. In that study, the difference between the nocebo rates and the respective rates of the active drug arms (active drug AE rate 61.6% and active drug dropout rate 8.8%) was very narrow. The univariate analysis showed that sample size, body mass index (BMI), and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score were potential factors negatively correlating to AEs and dropouts among the placebo-treated populations, whereas age was a potential factor positively correlating to AEs and dropouts among the placebo-treated populations. In addition year of publication was negatively correlating with nocebo AE rate, disease duration was positively correlating with nocebo AE rate, and male gender was negatively correlating with nocebo dropout rate. However, meta-regression analysis showed that only sample size was negatively correlated with both the nocebo AEs and dropout rates. 9.3.3 Parkinson’s Disease In a meta-analysis, which included 41 RCTs and was conducted by Stathis et al. [26], the nocebo AE rate in Parkinson’s disease (PD) was estimated to be 64.7%. The nocebo dropout rate was estimated to be 8.8%. In that study, the difference between the nocebo rates and the respective rates of the active drug arms (active drug AE rate 73.3% and active drug dropout rate 9.8%) was very narrow, showing that nocebo is a very important confounding factor of the reported AEs in RCTs as well as in the clinical practice when treating patients with AD. A very strong correlation was also found between the nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout rates and the active drug AEs and active drug dropout rates, respectively. 106 9.3.4 P. Zis Multiple Sclerosis Using a meta-analytic approach, Papadopoulos and Mitsikostas [20] estimated that the pooled incidence of nocebo responses is 74.4% in disease-modifying treatment (DMT) trials and 25.3% in symptomatic treatment (ST) trials in multiple sclerosis (MS). The pooled nocebo dropout rate is 2.1% in DMT and 2.3% in ST trials. Meta-regression analysis revealed a higher nocebo incidence in parallel design ST studies compared to crossover ones and a higher nocebo dropout rate in phase II ST studies compared to phase III ones. Nocebo dropout rate in DMT trials exhibited an association with the year of study publication and the frequency of drug administration. 9.3.5 Epilepsy Zaccara et al. have investigated the nocebo effect by analyzing placebo-treated, drug-resistant, focal epileptic patients in RCTs [31]. They estimated that the proportion of placebo-treated patients withdrawing because of AEs is 3.9%, and the proportion of patients with AEs is 60.3%. The drawback of this study was that the majority of the included RCTs were studies where placebo was compared to an active drug as an add-on treatment. Therefore, patients that were treated with placebo were already on one or more other antiepileptic drugs. In an attempt to estimate the nocebo effect in patients not receiving any active antiepileptic treatment, Zis et al. conducted a meta-analysis of four RCTs in refractory partial epilepsy during pre-surgical monitoring [33]. The authors reported that the pooled estimate of the percentage of placebo-treated patients who withdrew treatment was 76.8% and the pooled estimate of the percentage of placebo-treated patients who withdrew treatment because of AEs related to treatment (other than seizures, which were considered to be a disease-related event) was 3.2%. The pooled estimate of the percentage of active drug-treated patients who withdrew treatment was 52.1%, and the pooled estimate of the percentage of active drug-treated patients who withdrew treatment because of AEs related to treatment was 8.3%. 9.3.6 Motor Neuron Disease Motor neuron disease (MND) is a progressive degenerative disease, commonly affecting both the upper and the lower motor neurons. Thus, although it is not exclusively a brain disorder, MND can affect the motor cortex. A meta-analysis of 12 RCTs conducted by Shafiq et al. showed that approximately 8 in 10 placebo-treated patients (78.3%) report at least 1 AE, and approximately 1 in 12 placebo-treated patients discontinue placebo treatment because of 9 Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders 107 AEs other than death (8.4%). The respective rates for the active treatment groups are 84.7% and 12.6%. The fact that these differences are very narrow demonstrates that nocebo is an important confounding factor of reported AEs in MND trials [24]. 9.4 Placebo Effects in Non-traumatic Brain Disorders In the majority of RCTs for the treatment of major depressive disorder, at least one third of patients assigned to the placebo arm show clinically significant improvement [19]. It has been found that recovery from depression in placebo groups correlates with changes in frontal and cingulate cortical activity [29], suggesting that placebo is centrally driven. In depression, placebo and nocebo responses correlate significantly [18]. Similarly, in Alzheimer’s disease, Zis et al. showed that the nocebo AE incidence is positively correlated with the cognitive scores (using the ADAS-Cog tool), meaning that the more effective the placebo is, the more AEs and more dropouts occur [32]. In Parkinson’s disease, clinicians have been describing placebo effects in their patients for decades [13]. Significant placebo effects have been observed in pharmacological and non-pharmacological (i.e., deep brain stimulation and stem cell implantation) RCTs. Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that placebos stimulate the release of dopamine in the striatum of patients with Parkinson’s disease and can alter the activity of dopamine neurons [13]. In multiple sclerosis, it has been shown that patients on placebo show a decreased frequency of relapses during follow-up (in relapsing remitting cases) and decreased rate of progression of the disability (in progressive cases) compared to the pre-trial periods [12]. Although a possible explanation for this is the fact that in some RCTs the patients are selected based on their progression rate (selection bias) and the phenomenon of “regression toward the mean” can occur, an actual placebo effect is also very likely [12]. Interestingly, placebo effects have also been observed in immunological responses, such as the natural killer cell activity [11]. The possible mechanism for this is unknown; however it is unlikely that a clinical improvement driven by the placebo effect is related to the transient increase in NK cell activity [11]. In epilepsy, the placebo effect has also been observed in RCTs, as placebo interventions can reduce the seizure activity of the patients [8]. Some seizure reduction traditionally attributed to placebo effect, however, may reflect the natural course of the disease itself [8]. 9.5 Nature of AEs Across the Brain Disorders Table 9.2 summarizes the nature of the AEs across brain disorders other than headache, where meta-analyses have been performed to date. It is clear that across all disorders, the nature of AEs reported in the placebo-treated subjects mirrors those 108 P. Zis Table 9.2 Adverse events (AEs) reported in placebo-treated groups (nocebo AEs) and activetreated groups across brain disorders other than headache, where available Depression [18] Alzheimer’s disease [32] Parkinson’s disease [26] Epilepsy [33] Motor neuron disease [24] Placebo-treated patients Headache Nausea Somnolence/sedation Diarrhea Headache Urinary tract infection Nausea Dizziness Dyskinesia Headache Dizziness Fatigue/asthenia Respiratory disorders Dysphagia Active drug-treated patients Dizziness Somnolence/sedation Headache Diarrhea Headache Dizziness Nausea Dizziness Dyskinesia Headache Nausea Dizziness Respiratory disorders Fatigue/asthenia Dysphagia The three commonest AEs are mentioned in each disorder (most common on top) reported by active drug-treated subjects, suggesting that awareness of drug side effect profiles might have influenced patient expectations and, thus, nocebo responses [24]. However, the inherent difficulty in attributing non-specific symptoms (i.e., headache and dizziness) has to be recognized as a potential source of bias in these meta-analyses. An important observation is that it can be difficult to distinguish whether AEs arise subsequent to drug administration or as a consequence of disease worsening. In particular, dyskinesias reported in both the placebo and the active-treated patients with Parkinson’s disease, urinary tract infections reported in the placebo-treated patients with Alzheimer’s disease, and all the commonest AEs reported across both the placebo and the active-treated patients with MND may as well be part of the disease or the disease progression. Theoretically, the only way to estimate genuine nocebo rates would be by comparing a group of patients not treated at all with a group of patients treated with an inactive substance (placebo). The difference of the rates of new symptoms occurring during such a study, between the two arms, would be the true nocebo effect. However, since there are available treatments in these diseases, such studies are unethical and therefore unrealistic [24]. 9.6 Comparison Among Brain Disorders Although direct comparison with other neurological diseases is difficult because of the reasons related to trial populations, primary end points, severity, progression, and pathophysiology, it can give an overall picture of nocebo magnitude across the various brain disorders. 9 Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders 109 Table 9.3 Nocebo AE rates and nocebo dropout rates in all brain disorders where nocebo effect has been studied to date Disorder Motor neuron disease [24] Multiple sclerosis [20] Disease-modifying trials Symptomatic therapy trials Parkinson’s disease [26] Refractory partial epilepsy [33] Alzheimer’s disease [32] Depression [18] Headache [16] Preventative treatment Symptomatic treatment AE rate (%) 78.3 74.4 25.3 Dropout rate (%) 8.4 2.1 2.3 64.7 60.8 57.8 44.7 42.8 18.5 8.8 4.0 6.6 4.5 4.8 0.3 AE adverse event Table 9.3 summarizes the nocebo AE rates and nocebo dropout rates in all neurological disorders where nocebo effect was studied, using identical methodology. The highest nocebo dropout rate has been observed in Parkinson’s disease [26]. Human experimental evidence suggests that negative expectations could result in motor deterioration in patients with PD [23]. PET studies showed that high placebo responses were associated with greater dopamine (DA) and opioid activity in the nucleus accumbens, whereas nocebo responses were associated with a deactivation of DA and opioid release [23]. Both systems modulate a number of processes, including the regulation of reward and affective states. Thus, increased nocebo should be expected in PD, although DA replacement therapy results in changes in many aspects of neural activity within the entire basal ganglia cortical networks that are not yet fully understood [10, 27]. Not considering the nocebo dropout rate during symptomatic treatment for headache and MS, which are short-lived interventions, the lowest nocebo dropout rate has been observed during disease-modifying treatments in multiple sclerosis [20]. A possible explanation for this observation is that trial participants are not necessarily representative of the general population of MS patients as the wide range of available MS treatments today have reduced the pool of untreated, potentially recruitable patients for trials. Those MS patients who decide to participate in trials are likely to be more motivated and committed to adhere to the treatment regime [20]. The highest nocebo AE rate has been observed in MND [24]. Although AEs that are reported in RCTs are classified as drug-related, a potential source of bias is that it can be difficult to distinguish whether symptoms arise subsequent to placebo administration or as a consequence of disease worsening. Especially AEs of respiratory origin, which have been documented as drug-related in the respective RCTs, might as well be a disease complication per se [24]. The lowest nocebo AE rate has been observed during symptomatic treatment for headaches [16], closely followed by the nocebo AE rate during symptomatic treatment for multiple sclerosis. An interesting finding was that the nocebo AE rate 110 P. Zis during disease-modifying treatment is almost three times higher compared to symptomatic treatment for MS. This may reflect the fact that that meta-analysis included RCTs of already licensed drugs for symptomatic treatment [20]. 9.7 Directions for the Future Although the main brain disorders have been studied to date (headache, Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, depression, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, and motor neuron disease), there are still unexplored areas, which should be investigated further. These include psychiatric (i.e., schizophrenia and bipolar disorder), vascular (i.e., acute ischemic stroke, vascular dementia), degenerative (i.e., frontotemporal dementia, Lewy body dementia), and other systemic atrophies of the brain (i.e., hereditary ataxias). Studying nocebo in neoplastic and infectious brain disorders is an unrealistic aim since there are already available treatments for such disorders, and, therefore, conducting new RCTs with a pure placebo arm is raising ethical issues. The wide range of nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout rates across the disease suggests that nocebo is a disease-specific phenomenon. Nocebo is clearly affected by the underlying pathophysiology of the disease and the patients’ expectations and is strongly linked to the disease’s natural history and the availability of treatments. Therefore, meta-analyses of RCT of specific medications or group of medications (i.e., antiepileptics or antidepressants) that can target different disorders (i.e., neuropathic pain and epilepsy, neuropathic pain and depression) are of limited value. 9.8 Conclusions Nocebo has been found to be very prevalent in various brain disorders including headache, Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, depression, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, and motor neuron disease. There are still unexplored areas, which should be investigated further, given the significance of nocebo. The consequences of nocebo in clinical practice are important, as current treatments are based upon evidence confounded by the nocebo effect. The available meta-analyses do not provide any direct evidence for nocebo in clinical practice, but, as long as RCT findings predict the effectiveness and the safety of a treatment in clinical practice, the post hoc safety analyses may provide important considerations relevant to daily clinical practice as well. In any case, clinicians should be aware that drug intolerance and treatment failure might be caused by nocebo and recruit individualized strategies to limit it. Nocebo has, however, implications for RCT design. Modifying informed consents for the tested pharmaceutical agents to include the chance of nocebo, blinding 9 Placebos and Nocebos in Other Brain Disorders 111 the investigators to the analysis of recorded AEs, paying more attention to the AEs reported by the participants, and requesting more detailed safety reports by the investigators are potential interventions in future RCTs. References 1. Barsky AJ, Saintfort R, Rogers MP, Borus JF. Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon. JAMA. 2002;287(5):622–7. 2. Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When words are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience. 2007;147:260–71. 3. Burns PB, Rohrich RJ, Chung KC. The levels of evidence and their role in evidence-based medicine. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2011;128(1):305–10. 4. Dimitriadis PA, Zis P. Nocebo effect in Meniere’s disease: a meta-analysis of placebo-controlled randomized controlled trials. Otol Neurootol. 2017;38(9):1370–5. 5. Dodd S, Schacht A, Kelin K, Dueñas H, Reed VA, Williams LJ, Quirk FH, Malhi GS, Berk M. Nocebo effects in the treatment of major depression: results from an individual study participant-level meta-analysis of the placebo arm of duloxetine clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76(6):702–11. 6. Elsenbruch S, Schmid J, Bäsler M, Cesko E, Schedlowski M, Benson S. How positive and negative expectations shape the experience of visceral pain: an experimental pilot study in healthy women. Neurogastroenterol Motil. 2012;24(10):914–e460. 7. Enck P, Benedetti F, Schedlowski M. New insights into the placebo and nocebo responses. Neuron. 2008;59:195–206. 8. Goldenholz DM, Moss R, Scott J, Auh S, Theodore WH. Confusing placebo effect with natural history in epilepsy: a big data approach. Ann Neurol. 2015;78(3):329–36. 9. Häuser W, Bartram C, Bartram-Wunn E, Tölle T. Adverse events attributable to nocebo in randomized controlled drug trials in fibromyalgia syndrome and painful diabetic peripheral neuropathy: systematic review. Clin J Pain. 2012;28(5):437–51. 10. Heimer G, Rivlin-Etzion M, Bar-Gad I, Goldberg JA, Haber SN, Bergman H. Dopamine replacement therapy does not restore the full spectrum of normal pallidal activity in the 1-Methyl-4-Phenyl-1,2,3,6-Tetra-Hydropyridine primate model of Parkinsonism. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8101–14. 11. Hirsch RL, Johnson KP, Camenga DL. The placebo effect during a double blind trial of recombinant alpha 2 interferon in multiple sclerosis patients: immunological and clinical findings. Int J Neurosci. 1988;39(3–4):189–96. 12. La Mantia L, Eoli M, Salmaggi A, Milanese C. Does a placebo-effect exist in clinical trials on multiple sclerosis? Review of the literature. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1996;17(2):135–9. 13. Lidstone SC. Great expectations: the placebo effect in Parkinson’s disease. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2014;225:139–47. 14. Manchikanti L, Giordano J, Fellows B, Hirsch JA. Placebo and nocebo in interventional pain management: a friend or a foe—or simply foes? Pain Physician. 2011;14(2):E157–75. 15. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headaches: implications for clinical practice and trial design. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2012;12(2):132–7. 16. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis LI, Chalarakis NG. Nocebo is the enemy, not placebo. A meta-analysis of reported side effects after placebo treatment in headaches. Cephalalgia. 2011;31(5):550–61. 17. Mitsikostas DD, Chalarakis NG, Mantonakis LI, Delicha EM, Sfikakis PP. Nocebo in fibromyalgia: meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials and implications for practice. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(5):672–80. 112 P. Zis 18. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis L, Chalarakis N. Nocebo in clinical trials for depression: a metaanalysis. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(1):82–6. 19. Nehama Y, Rabinowitz I, Baruch Y, Mandel A, Lurie I, Barak Y. Debunking the placebo effect in depression: the effect of patient and investigator expectation on escitalopram efficacy. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;29(2):106–10. 20. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effects in multiple sclerosis trials: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler. 2010;16(7):816–28. 21. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. A meta-analytic approach to estimating nocebo effects in neuropathic pain trials. J Neurol. 2012;259(3):436–47. 22. Rutherford BR, Wall MM, Glass A, Stewart JW. The role of patient expectancy in placebo and nocebo effects in antidepressant trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;75(10):1040–6. 23. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):220–31. 24. Shafiq F, Mitsikostas DD, Zis P. Nocebo in motor neuron disease: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener. 2017;8:1–7. 25. Silva MA, Duarte GS, Camara R, Rodrigues FB, Fernandes RM, Abreu D, Mestre T, Costa J, Trenkwalder C, Ferreira JJ. Placebo and nocebo responses in restless legs syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2017;88(23):2216–24. 26. Stathis P, Smpiliris M, Konitsiotis S, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo as a potential confounding factor in clinical trials for Parkinson’s disease treatment: a meta-analysis. Eur J Neurol. 2013;20(3):527–33. 27. The Parkinson Study Group. Levodopa and the progression of Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:2498–508. 28. Tracey I. Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nat Med. 2010;16(11):1277–83. 29. Vallance AK. A systematic review comparing the functional neuroanatomy of patients with depression who respond to placebo to those who recover spontaneously: is there a biological basis for the placebo effect in depression? J Affect Disord. 2007;98(1–2):177–85. 30. World Health Organization. International classification of diseases, 10th revision (ICD-10). Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. 31. Zaccara G, Giovannelli F, Cincotta M, Loiacono G, Verrotti A. Adverse events of placebotreated, drug-resistant, focal epileptic patients in randomized controlled trials: a systematic review. J Neurol. 2015;262(3):501–15. 32. Zis P, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in Alzheimer’s disease; meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. J Neurol Sci. 2015;355(1–2):94–100. 33. Zis P, Shafiq F, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effect in refractory partial epilepsy during pre-surgical monitoring: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. Seizure. 2017;45:95–9. Chapter 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research Luana Colloca and Nathaniel Haycock 10.1 he Dilemma of Clinical Trial Designs T and Placebo Effects Distinguishing placebo responses from placebo effects is the key to avoid confusion in interpreting findings and advance clinical research designs and outcomes [21]. These terms often appear in published articles interchangeably but they do not have the same meaning. When a drug is given, the effectiveness of a treatment can be determined by comparing the changes observed in patients receiving the drug (treatment response) with those receiving placebo (placebo response). The placebo effect—effect of expectancy in clinical outcomes—is the difference between the placebo and no-intervention arm allows scientists to account for any changes that would have been observed without any treatment (natural history) or through spontaneous remission, regression to mean, and the Hawthorne effect [23]. The placebo effect describes any improvements that are over and above these nonspecific, confounding changes that may occur in a no-intervention arm. Thus, in order to talk about the placebo effect, we need clinical trials that include a L. Colloca (*) Department of Pain and Translational Symptom Science, University of Maryland School of Nursing, Baltimore, MD, USA Departments of Anesthesiology and Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA Center to Advance Chronic Pain Research, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, USA Program in Neuroscience, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA e-mail: [email protected]; [email protected] N. Haycock Department of Pain and Translational Symptom Science, University of Maryland School of Nursing, Baltimore, MD, USA © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_10 113 114 L. Colloca and N. Haycock no-intervention arm along with a placebo arm in order to distinguish changes seen in an untreated group from the treated arms. Although the no-intervention arm is one way to estimate the placebo effect, it is often quite difficult to propose the kind of clinical trial design in which waiting list arms are included as the control group for placebo effects. Alternative clinical trial designs such as the sequential parallel comparison design can bring the need to disentangle placebo effects into harmony with ethical principles of research [34]. Another approach that may help detect placebo effects in the context of clinical trials (and practice) is a systematic assessment of participants’ expectations. There is evidence that expectations (e.g., of pain relief) influence a variety of medical conditions [9, 37]. For example, patients’ expectations can positively influence both long-term mortality [8] and response to surgical interventions [7]. They can also potentially predict long-term postoperative walking performance following hip and knee arthroplasty [74] as well as patients’ satisfaction following total joint arthroplasty [53]. Laboratory research has demonstrated that expectations, which are often shaped by open-hidden paradigms, can impact the response to medication such as morphine, anxiolytic diazepam, deep brain stimulation [for a review, see [25]], i.v. remifentanil [14], topical lidocaine/capsaicin [62], and acupuncture [51]. Therefore, it is very useful to measure expectations and related factors (e.g., credibility and perception of effectiveness). Measures for expectations range from single questions to multi-item scales. One common question is: “What do you expect your level of pain intensity to be?” [75]. Questions such as this can be corroborated with other single questions dealing with credibility of the clinician and treatment, such as “How effective do you think this treatment will be?” [29]. Another important measurement to collect is perceived effectiveness of the treatment [see, [41, 61]]. Three items such as these are easy to administer to placebo and medication arms within clinical trials. It remains problematic, however, to assess them in a natural history arm. 10.2 rug and Placebo Effects: Additive D and Interaction Effects The relationship between drug and placebo responses (and placebo effects if the natural history is included) is highly relevant for clinical research given that the gold standard of efficacy in randomized clinical trials, when drug responses outperform placebo responses, is based on the specious assumption that drug and placebo responses are additive [see 17 for a review]. A 2 × 2 balanced placebo design with instructions about the drug (told drug versus told placebo) as one factor and actual drug (given drug versus given placebo) as the other factor can allow to test interactions between drug and placebo responses. This design, in combination with neuroimaging, has been helpful to explore both additive and interaction effects [42, 76, 77]. 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research 115 Both additive and interaction effects between drugs and placebo responses have been reported. Significant fMRI activity associated with the main effect of instructions as well as an apparent interaction with treatment in the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus and left medial frontal gyrus has been found by Kong et al. [47], who used verbal instruction (positive instruction vs. neutral instruction) alongside acupuncture (real vs. sham). A balanced placebo and an open-hidden design were used by Atlas et al. [6] to separate the effect of remifentanil and instruction in a pharmacokinetic model. Remifentanil and instructions both reduced pain ratings, but the effect of remifentanil on pain reports and fMRI activity did not interact with verbally induced changes. In contrast, the effect of lidocaine/prilocaine on pain ratings and brain signaling in the anterior insula rACC, the anterior insula, and the ventral striatum showed an interactive effect in a study by Schenk et al. [69] using fMRI. The study utilized a within-subjects design and balanced placebo (received lidocaine/prilocaine versus received control cream) with an instruction manipulation (told lidocaine/prilocaine versus told control cream). Clinical findings have also suggested that placebo and drug effects may not be merely additive [25]. The additive versus interactive effect may depend on the mechanisms of action of the given treatment. Future research with in vivo receptorial radiotracers and PET imaging techniques (e.g., PET with carfentanil radiotracers) may help illustrate this aspect with relevant implication for clinical and translational research. 10.3 Silencing the DLPC to Reduce Placebo Effects Placebo- and nocebo-pain signaling have been associated with brain regions such as the thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (S1/S2), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and insula [3, 64, 65]. Namely, reduced activity has been documented in the ACC, insula, and thalamus [32, 66, 78]. Further, a meta-analysis of fMRI placebo and pain studies has identified the insula, dorsal ACC, thalamus, amygdala, and right lateral prefrontal cortex as less active during placebo analgesic effects [5] with a few trends in opposite directions [46]. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been linked to the generation and maintenance of placebo analgesia over time [48, 52, 78, 80, 82]. Anticipation of placebo analgesia seen during fMRI in the DLPFC, which is involved in emotion regulation [56], working memory [63], and cognitive control [54], correlates with the magnitude of individual placebo effects [52, 78]. Evidence of the role of the DLPFC also stems from experiments using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to momentarily silence the function of left and right DLPFC [48] and from a study with impairment of the DLPFC in patients with Alzheimer’s disease that had loss of prefrontal executive functions [13]. Importantly, these studies suggest that it is potentially feasible to modulate placebo and nocebo effects by changing the transient excitability of the rDLPFC using tDCS [31] and TMS. This approach can be of help to advance research on treatment efficacy since silencing the DLPFC can minimize placebo effects. 116 10.4 L. Colloca and N. Haycock he Inner Pharmacy: Potentials for Future T Biomarkers? Studies suggest that distinct systems, namely, the dopamine, opioid, serotonin, endocannabinoid, oxytocin, vasopressin, and cholecystokinin systems, influence placebo and nocebo effects. The most robust and well-established notion is that the opioid system is heavily involved in the formation of placebo analgesic effects [1, 32, 50, 58]. Pioneering studies have demonstrated that placebo analgesia is antagonized by the opioid antagonist naloxone [1, 32, 50]. Functional coupling of the rostral ACC (rACC) and the periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Eippert, Bingel et al. 2009) was reduced when naloxone (0.15 mg/kg) was administered before the test phase of a study with a classical conditioning paradigm using placebo. Opioid signaling has been well-documented with in vivo human PET studies and the μ-opioid receptor-selective radiotracer [11C]carfentanil studies [57, 61, 70, 79, 82]. The activation of opioid neurotransmission has been explored along with the dopaminergic system during a placebo administration using carbon 11 [11C]-labeled raclopride (and [11C] carfentanil) in the PET [70]. Despite negative results in shaping placebo analgesia using dopamine antagonist haloperidol (2 mg) and the agonist levodopa/carbidopa (100/25 mg) to test the involvement of dopamine [71], dopaminergic activation prevailed in the nucleus accumbens and accounted for 25% of the variance in placebo responders and nonresponders [70]. The concept of placebo responders and nonresponders may sound controversial (e.g., is a person a responder to a variety of treatments and manipulations); however being able to split study participants into responders and nonresponders has advantages for describing behavioral, objective, and clinical phenotypes. Recently, the CB1 receptor antagonist, rimonabant, has been used to block conditioned analgesic effects when non-opioid pharmacological conditioning with the nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drug (NSAID) ketorolac is performed [11], which suggests an involvement of the cannabinoid system. By blocking the CCK A and B receptors with the nonselective A/B receptor antagonist proglumide, placebo analgesia can be enhanced [10] probably by reversing nocebo-induced hyperalgesia [12]. The concept of enhancing placebo effects opens up new approaches that leverage the use of agonists. Oxytocin agonists given intranasally enhanced placebo analgesia in men [43], and a nonselective vasopressin agonist for both Avp1a and Avp1b receptors enhanced placebo effects in women [27]. Avp1a and Avp1b vasopressin receptors largely expressed within the central nervous system regulate social behaviors, stress, conciliatory behaviors [35, 68], and social communication ­ responses [72, 73]. Pharmacological studies indicate the existence of a sort of “inner pharmacy” that is in turn activated to trigger placebo and nocebo effects. Future efforts should be made to understand the contribution of specific receptor expressions and functions within each system using selective antagonists and agonists and potential biomarkers. This knowledge is guiding research on the role of distinct genetic variants (see next section). 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research 10.5 117 I s There a Genetic Biomarker of Placebo Analgesic Effects? Emerging studies on “the placebome,” or the genetic variants that influence the placebo effect [18, 40], promise to increase our understanding of the mechanisms underlying placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia and potentially help advance clinical research. In fact, understanding how specific genetic variants influence the placebo effect may allow clinical researchers to tailor treatments and study design (e.g., randomized clinical trials) to individuals to maximize outcomes and to disentangle treatment effects from placebo effects. Namely, results from studies relating SNPs of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene [39, 81], the monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A) X-linked gene [49], the dopamine beta-hydroxylase (DBH) gene [4], and the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene [60] provide evidence that polymorphisms which reduce dopaminergic activity may be linked with decreased placebo response. Genetic variants that affect dopamine pathways appear to affect the placebo effect and may eventually serve as biomarkers to differentiate placebo responders from nonresponders. Serotoninergic pathway genes (e.g., TPH2, 5-HTTLPR, 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter SLC6A4 SNP rs4251417, HTR2A SNPs rs2296972 and rs622337) may also be involved in placebo analgesia [33, 36], although further research is needed to provide conclusive evidence. In addition, studies by Pecina and colleagues on analgesia [59] suggest that genes involved in the cannabinoid and opioid systems may be two of the strongest mediators of placebo responses and effects. The investigators found that the magnitude of placebo analgesia and improved mood was greatest for individuals homozygous for the common Pro129/Pro129 genotype of the fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH) gene and directly linked the opioid system with the cannabinoid system in the context of placebo analgesia suggesting that different systems may play a role when a placebo analgesic effect is observed. So far, the strongest evidence linking genetic variants with the placebo effect comes from studies of opioidergic pathways and placebo analgesia. For example, aspartic acid (G) allele carriers for the rs1799971 functional polymorphism in the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) showed reduced opioid receptor expression, ­function, and density as well as lower placebo effects, in the nucleus accumbens during placebo analgesia [58]. In that same study, using positron emission tomography (PET) and selective radiotracers to label μ-opioid and dopamine receptors (D2/D3), Pecina et al. found that AA homozygotes had greater baseline availability of μ-opioid receptors than G allele carriers in the anterior insula, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, thalamus, and brainstem. Interestingly, G allele carriers demonstrated higher NEO-Neuroticism personality scores indicating that exploring psychological traits along with genetic variants may help identify critical phenotypes of placebo responders and nonresponders [57]. Clearly, variation in OPRM1 is involved with individual differences in response to pain and placebo. 118 L. Colloca and N. Haycock Despite these results, the existence of many mechanisms underlying placebo effects suggests that it is unlikely that single polymorphisms can, by themselves, explain why placebo analgesia and hyperalgesia occur. However, these correlations do promise to uncover a set of common alleles that may interact to influence complex traits related to placebo effects. 10.6 atient-Doctor Communication and Nocebo Effects: P A Self-Fulfilling Prophecy In clinical research, nonspecific symptoms and complaints in patient populations, medication nonadherence, and need for additional drug prescriptions are often related to nocebo effects. For example, the mere mention of headaches as a common side effect during the informed consent process of studies on antidepressants and other medications can increase the likelihood that headaches are experienced during the study [15, 19, 20, 45]. Informing study participants about the occurrence of adverse effects leads to withdrawal from the study. For example, mentioning gastrointestinal side effects during the consent process in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial that examined the effects of either aspirin, sulfinpyrazone, or both drugs for the treatment of unstable angina pectoris induced a sixfold increase of gastrointestinal symptoms and elicited consequent patient-initiated cessation of therapies [55]. Discontinuation and lack of adherence to statin drugs have been also reported in population-based studies. In statin trials performed from 1994 to 2003, placebo arms showed a variety of symptoms including (but not limited to) headache (0.2– 2.7%) and abdominal pain (0.9–3.9%). These adverse events were even higher in the general population when statins moved from phase III to phase IV [67]. The large lipid-lowering arm of the Anglo-Scandinavian Cardiac Outcomes Trial recently published showed that 10 mg open-label atorvastatin and placebo induced an excess rate of muscle-related adverse events in the non-blinded non-randomized 3-year follow-up phase but not in the blinded initial 5-year phase. In the non-blinded phase, public claims about side effects may have caused patients and physicians to expect to experience the alerted adverse events [38]. Nocebo effects thus contribute to the occurrence of side effects and shape both clinical outcomes and patients’ adherence to medication [24]. Preventing deleterious communication while still protecting patients’ rights and preferences is therefore critical. Since medical-informed consent documents have great potential to modify patient expectations, empirical work has been conducted regarding risk and benefit communication, with special emphasis on the effect of different framing strategies on shared decision-making processes. Benefits and risks can be provided verbally or numerically [16]. Information can be framed as relative, as opposed to absolute risks or “numbers needed to treat (NNT)”, in a way that appears convincing and, in the clinical context, may facilitate adherence [30]. Moreover, risks or benefits can be framed 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research 119 as gains or losses. Accordingly, negatively framed side effects focus on the number of patients experiencing a given risk (e.g., 38 out of 100 study participants taking antidepressants will experience headache), while positively framed information focuses on the number of participants not experiencing the adverse effect (e.g., 62 out of 100 participants will not experience headache). It seems intuitively plausible that the second wording will lead to a less negative-risk perception. However, the negative framing remains the current standard for informed consent procedures [28]. Clearly, framing effects may be moderated by psychological factors including the recipient (participant)’s disposition and prior therapeutic experiences (see below). 10.7 I mpact of Positive and Negative Prior Therapeutic Experiences Positive and negative previous therapeutic experiences may not only confound the results of crossover designs but also shape expectancies and treatment responses. Learned placebo and nocebo effects can be elicited in healthy participants as well as in participants suffering from pain and other disorders. Prior analgesic experiences can increase the response to a subsequent placebo, and negative hyperalgesic experiences can decrease the magnitude of placebo effects. Colloca and Benedetti [22] performed a study in which one group received a placebo intervention after being exposed to an intensity of painful stimulations that was surreptitiously decreased and a second group received a placebo after another treatment was made ineffective (e.g., no manipulation of the intensity of painful stimulation was performed). Both groups were tested for placebo analgesic effects after a time lag of 4–7 days. Placebo effects following the effective procedure were remarkably higher than those following the ineffective treatment. These findings suggest that placebo effects may be moderated by prior experience (either positive or negative) and that the effect of initial treatment does influence the magnitude of subsequent placebo effects even days after being exposed to them [22]. Similarly, Kessner et al. introduced a new analgesic treatment after randomization of healthy study participants to two arms exposed to effective and ineffective (actually always inert) patch treatment, to test for the effect of treatment history [44]. As anticipated, the therapeutic response to the tested treatment was lower in the negative compared to the positive treatment history arm. The negative treatment history induced a higher activation of the bilateral posterior INS, and a lower activation of the right DLPFC, which is involved in inhibitory modulatory processes for placebo as described above [44]. There is a relationship between the effect of prior positive and negative experiences and the magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects. The greater the exposure to negative and positive treatment experiences, the higher the nocebo and placebo effects, respectively [26]. Similar results have been observed in pain patients. André-Obadia et al. demonstrated in a crossover study design aimed at evaluating 120 L. Colloca and N. Haycock the therapeutic efficacy of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) that the size of placebo analgesia in patients suffering from chronic neuropathic pain depends on prior experience of either successful or unsuccessful treatments [2]. These findings deserve further investigation for the potential to improve the design of clinical trials and, likewise, optimize therapeutic strategies. 10.8 Conclusions Knowledge of placebo and nocebo responses can help improve clinical research. By incorporating natural history groups as well as placebo groups into RCTs and keeping individual differences in placebo effects in mind, researchers can control a variety of confounds including natural history and regression to the mean, among others. Doing so will lead to an increase in the design of more rigorous, reliable, and valid studies. Beyond controlling for confounding factors, knowing what individual and contextual factors (e.g., prior experiences effects and framing effects) contribute to placebos and nocebos can enable researchers to harness those effects to yield optimal treatment outcomes. Acknowledgments Work reported here was supported by grants of the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (R01DE025946 to LC) of the National Institutes of Health. This work was supported also by the MPowering the State, a grant from the State of Maryland (LC). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. The authors report no conflict of interest. The funding sources had no role in study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; the writing of the report; or the decision to submit the chapter for publication. References 1. Amanzio M, Benedetti F. Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J Neurosci. 1999;19(1):484–94. 2. Andre-Obadia N, Magnin M, Garcia-Larrea L. On the importance of placebo timing in rTMS studies for pain relief. Pain. 2011;152(6):1233–7. 3. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede R-D, Zubieta J-K. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain. 2005;9(4):463. 4. Arias AJ, Gelernter J, Gueorguieva R, Ralevski E, Petrakis IL. Pharmacogenetics of naltrexone and disulfiram in alcohol dependent, dually diagnosed veterans. Am J Addict. 2014;23(3):288–93. 5. Atlas LY, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of brain mechanisms of placebo analgesia: consistent findings and unanswered questions. In: Benedetti F, Enck P, Frisaldi E, Schedlowski M, editors. Placebo. Berlin: Springer; 2014. p. 37–69. 6. Atlas LY, Whittington RA, Lindquist MA, Wielgosz J, Sonty N, Wager TD. Dissociable influences of opiates and expectations on pain. J Neurosci. 2012;32(23):8053–64. 7. Auer CJ, Glombiewski JA, Doering BK, Winkler A, Laferton JA, Broadbent E, Rief W. Patients’ expectations predict surgery outcomes: a meta-analysis. Int J Behav Med. 2016;23(1):49–62. 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research 121 8. Barefoot JC, Brummett BH, Williams RB, Siegler IC, Helms MJ, Boyle SH, Clapp-Channing NE, Mark DB. Recovery expectations and long-term prognosis of patients with coronary heart disease. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171(10):929–35. 9. Benedetti F. Mechanisms of placebo and placebo-related effects across diseases and treatments. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008;48:33–60. 10. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Maggi G. Potentiation of placebo analgesia by proglumide. Lancet. 1995;346(8984):1231. 11. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Rosato R, Blanchard C. Nonopioid placebo analgesia is mediated by CB1 cannabinoid receptors. Nat Med. 2011;17(10):1228–30. 12. Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Vighetti S, Asteggiano G. The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J Neurosci. 2006;26(46):12014–22. 13. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Costa S, Vighetti S, Tarenzi L, Rainero I, Asteggiano G. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer’s disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain. 2006;121(1–2):133–44. 14. Bingel U, Wanigasekera V, Wiech K, Ni Mhuircheartaigh R, Lee MC, Ploner M, Tracey I. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci Transl Med. 2011;3(70):70ra14. 15. Blasini M, Corsi N, Klinger R, Colloca L. Nocebo and pain: an overview of the psychoneurobiological mechanisms. Pain Rep. 2017;2(2):e585. 16. Buchter RB, Fechtelpeter D, Knelangen M, Ehrlich M, Waltering A. Words or numbers? Communicating risk of adverse effects in written consumer health information: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2014;14:76. 17. Colagiuri B. Participant expectancies in double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trials: potential limitations to trial validity. Clin Trials. 2010;7:246–55. 18. Colagiuri B, Schenk LA, Kessler MD, Dorsey SG, Colloca L. The placebo effect: from concepts to genes. Neuroscience. 2015;307:171–90. 19. Colloca L. Nocebo effects can make you feel pain. Science. 2017;358(6359):44. 20. Colloca L. Tell me the truth and I will not be harmed: informed consents and Nocebo effects. Am J Bioeth. 2017;17(6):46–8. 21. Colloca L. Treatment of pediatric migraine. N Engl J Med. 2017;376(14):1387–8. 22. Colloca L, Benedetti F. How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia. Pain. 2006;124(1–2):126–33. 23. Colloca L, Benedetti F, Porro CA. Experimental designs and brain mapping approaches for studying the placebo analgesic effect. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2008;102(4):371–80. 24. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA. 2012;307(6):567–8. 25. Colloca L, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Benedetti F. Overt versus covert treatment for pain, anxiety, and Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3(11):679–84. 26. Colloca L, Petrovic P, Wager TD, Ingvar M, Benedetti F. How the number of learning trials affects placebo and nocebo responses. Pain. 2010;151(2):430–9. 27. Colloca L, Pine DS, Ernst M, Miller FG, Grillon C. Vasopressin boosts placebo analgesic effects in women: a randomized trial. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(10):794–802. 28. Covey J. The role of dispositional factors in moderating message framing effects. Health Psychol. 2014;33(1):52–65. 29. Devilly GJ, Borkovec TD. Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2000;31(2):73–86. 30. Edwards A, Elwyn G, Covey J, Matthews E, Pill R. Presenting risk information—a review of the effects of “framing” and other manipulations on patient outcomes. J Health Commun. 2001;6(1):61–82. 31. Egorova N, Yu R, Kaur N, Vangel M, Gollub RL, Dougherty DD, Kong J, Camprodon JA. Neuromodulation of conditioned placebo/nocebo in heat pain: anodal vs cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Pain. 2015;156(7):1342–7. 122 L. Colloca and N. Haycock 32. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Buchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63(4):533–43. 33. Faria V, Appel L, Ahs F, Linnman C, Pissiota A, Frans O, Bani M, Bettica P, Pich EM, Jacobsson E, Wahlstedt K, Fredrikson M, Furmark T. Amygdala subregions tied to SSRI and placebo response in patients with social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(10):2222–32. 34. Fava M, Evins AE, Dorer DJ, Schoenfeld DA. The problem of the placebo response in clinical trials for psychiatric disorders: culprits, possible remedies, and a novel study design approach. Psychother Psychosom. 2003;72(3):115–27. 35. Feng C, Hackett PD, DeMarco AC, Chen X, Stair S, Haroon E, Ditzen B, Pagnoni G, Rilling JK. Oxytocin and vasopressin effects on the neural response to social cooperation are modulated by sex in humans. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;9(4):754–64. 36. Furmark T, Appel L, Henningsson S, Ahs F, Faria V, Linnman C, Pissiota A, Frans O, Bani M, Bettica P, Pich EM, Jacobsson E, Wahlstedt K, Oreland L, Langstrom B, Eriksson E, Fredrikson M. A link between serotonin-related gene polymorphisms, amygdala activity, and placeboinduced relief from social anxiety. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2008;28(49):13066–74. 37. Gramling R, Epstein R. Optimism amid serious disease: clinical panacea or ethical conundrum?: comment on “Recovery expectations and long-term prognosis of patients with coronary heart disease”. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171(10):935–6. 38. Gupta A, Thompson D, Whitehouse A, Collier T, Dahlof B, Poulter N, Collins R, Sever P, Investigators A. Adverse events associated with unblinded, but not with blinded, statin therapy in the Anglo-Scandinavian cardiac outcomes trial-lipid-lowering arm (ASCOT-LLA): a randomised double-blind placebo-controlled trial and its non-randomised non-blind extension phase. Lancet. 2017;389(10088):2473–81. 39. Hall KT, Lembo AJ, Kirsch I, Ziogas DC, Douaiher J, Jensen KB, Conboy LA, Kelley JM, Kokkotou E, Kaptchuk TJ. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met polymorphism predicts placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e48135. 40. Hall KT, Loscalzo J, Kaptchuk TJ. Genetics and the placebo effect: the placebome. Trends Mol Med. 2015;21(5):285–94. 41. Jarcho JM, Feier NA, Labus JS, Naliboff B, Smith SR, Hong JY, Colloca L, Tillisch K, Mandelkern MA, Mayer EA, London ED. Placebo analgesia: self-report measures and preliminary evidence of cortical dopamine release associated with placebo response. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;10:107–14. 42. Keltner JR, Furst A, Fan C, Redfern R, Inglis B, Fields HL. Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2006;26(16):4437–43. 43. Kessner S, Sprenger C, Wrobel N, Wiech K, Bingel U. Effect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia: a randomized study. JAMA. 2013;310(16):1733–5. 44. Kessner S, Wiech K, Forkmann K, Ploner M, Bingel U. The effect of treatment history on therapeutic outcome: an experimental approach. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(15):1468–9. 45. Klinger R, Blasini M, Schmitz J, Colloca L. Nocebo effects in clinical studies: hints for pain therapy. Pain Rep. 2017;2(2):586. 46. Kong J, Gollub RL, Rosman IS, Webb JM, Vangel MG, Kirsch I, Kaptchuk TJ. Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2006;26(2):381–8. 47. Kong J, Kaptchuk TJ, Polich G, Kirsch I, Vangel M, Zyloney C, Rosen B, Gollub RL. An fMRI study on the interaction and dissociation between expectation of pain relief and acupuncture treatment. NeuroImage. 2009;47(3):1066–76. 48. Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schonbachler G. Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;148(3):368–74. 49. Leuchter AF, McCracken JT, Hunter AM, Cook IA, Alpert JE. Monoamine oxidase a and catechol-o-methyltransferase functional polymorphisms and the placebo response in major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;29(4):372–7. 10 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Research 123 50. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL. The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet. 1978;2(8091):654–7. 51. Linde K, Witt CM, Streng A, Weidenhammer W, Wagenpfeil S, Brinkhaus B, Willich SN, Melchart D. The impact of patient expectations on outcomes in four randomized controlled trials of acupuncture in patients with chronic pain. Pain. 2007;128(3):264–71. 52. Lui F, Colloca L, Duzzi D, Anchisi D, Benedetti F, Porro CA. Neural bases of conditioned placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;151(3):816–24. 53. Mahomed NN, Liang MH, Cook EF, Daltroy LH, Fortin PR, Fossel AH, Katz JN. The importance of patient expectations in predicting functional outcomes after total joint arthroplasty. J Rheumatol. 2002;29(6):1273–9. 54. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24(1):167–202. 55. Myers MG, Cairns JA, Singer J. The consent form as a possible cause of side effects. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1987;42(3):250–3. 56. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(5):242–9. 57. Pecina M, Azhar H, Love TM, Lu T, Fredrickson BL, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK. Personality trait predictors of placebo analgesia and neurobiological correlates. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(4):639–46. 58. Pecina M, Love T, Stohler CS, Goldman D, Zubieta JK. Effects of the mu opioid receptor polymorphism (OPRM1 A118G) on pain regulation, placebo effects and associated personality trait measures. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(4):957–65. 59. Pecina M, Martinez-Jauand M, Hodgkinson C, Stohler CS, Goldman D, Zubieta JK. FAAH selectively influences placebo effects. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(3):385–91. 60. Peciña M, Martínez-Jauand M, Love T, Heffernan J, Montoya P, Hodgkinson C, Stohler CS, Goldman D, Zubieta J-K. Valence-specific effects of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on dopaminergic stress and reward processing in humans. J Neurosci. 2014;34(17):5874–81. 61. Pecina M, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK. Role of mu-opioid system in the formation of memory of placebo responses. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(2):135–7. 62. Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Grosen K, Pilegaard HK, Tracey I, Benedetti F, Price DD, Jensen TS, Vase L. Expectations and positive emotional feelings accompany reductions in ongoing and evoked neuropathic pain following placebo interventions. Pain. 2014;155(12): 2687–98. 63. Petrides M. The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. In: Schneider WX, Owen AM, Duncan J, editors. Executive control and the frontal lobe: current issues. Berlin: Springer; 2000. p. 44–54. 64. Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiol Clin. 2000;30(5):263–88. 65. Price DD, Barrell JJ. Mechanisms of analgesia produced by hypnosis and placebo suggestions. Prog Brain Res. 2000;122:255–71. 66. Price DD, Craggs J, Nicholas Verne G, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME. Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients. Pain. 2007;127(1–2):63–72. 67. Rief W, Avorn J, Barsky AJ. Medication-attributed adverse effects in placebo groups: implications for assessment of adverse effects. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(2):155–60. 68. Rilling JK, Demarco AC, Hackett PD, Chen X, Gautam P, Stair S, Haroon E, Thompson R, Ditzen B, Patel R, Pagnoni G. Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;39:237–48. 69. Schenk LA, Sprenger C, Geuter S, Büchel C. Expectation requires treatment to boost pain relief: an fMRI study. Pain. 2014;155(1):150–7. 70. Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):220–31. 124 L. Colloca and N. Haycock 71. Skyt I, Moslemi K, Baastrup C, Grosen K, Benedetti F, Petersen GL, Price DD, Hall KT, Kaptchuk TJ, Svensson P, Jensen TS, Vase L. Dopaminergic tone does not influence pain levels during placebo interventions in patients with chronic neuropathic pain. Pain. 2018;159(2):261–72. 72. Thompson R, Gupta S, Miller K, Mills S, Orr S. The effects of vasopressin on human facial responses related to social communication. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(1):35–48. 73. Thompson RR, George K, Walton JC, Orr SP, Benson J. Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(20):7889–94. 74. van den Akker-Scheek I, Stevens M, Groothoff JW, Bulstra SK, Zijlstra W. Preoperative or postoperative self-efficacy: which is a better predictor of outcome after total hip or knee arthroplasty? Patient Educ Couns. 2007;66(1):92–9. 75. Vase L, Robinson ME, Verne GN, Price DD. The contributions of suggestion, desire, and expectation to placebo effects in irritable bowel syndrome patients. An empirical investigation. Pain. 2003;105(1–2):17–25. 76. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Wong C, Jayne M, Telang F, Swanson JM. Effects of expectation on the brain metabolic responses to methylphenidate and to its placebo in nondrug abusing subjects. NeuroImage. 2006;32(4):1782–92. 77. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Zhu W, Maynard L, Telang F, Vaska P, Ding YS, Wong C, Swanson JM. Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and the reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers. J Neurosci. 2003;23(36):11461–8. 78. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303(5661):1162–7. 79. Wager TD, Scott DJ, Zubieta J-K. Placebo effects on human μ-opioid activity during pain. PNAS. 2007;104(26):11056–61. 80. Watson A, El-Deredy W, Iannetti GD, Lloyd D, Tracey I, Vogt BA, Nadeau V, Jones AKP. Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain anticipation and perception. Pain. 2009;145(1–2):24–30. 81. Yu R, Gollub RL, Vangel M, Kaptchuk T, Smoller JW, Kong J. Placebo analgesia and reward processing: integrating genetics, personality, and intrinsic brain activity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(9):4583–93. 82. Zubieta J-K, Bueller JA, Jackson LR, Scott DJ, Xu Y, Koeppe RA, Nichols TE, Stohler CS. Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on μ-opioid receptors. J Neurosci. 2005;25(34):7754–62. Chapter 11 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice Dimos D. Mitsikostas 11.1 Introduction The shadowy phenomenon known as nocebo describes negative expectancies for medical treatment resulting in experience of unpleasant symptoms. In contrast, positive expectancies trigger placebo that results in treatment outcome improvement. In evolutionary terms, nocebo and placebo coexist in humans to favor perceptual mechanisms that anticipate threat and dangerous events (nocebo) or promote appetitive and safety behaviors (placebo) [10, 11]. In clinical trials placebo and nocebo are powered mainly by pre-trial suggestions’ positive and negative ones, respectively, all of them delivered during the informed consent process [9]. In clinical practice however, multiple factors, both internal and environmental, encompassed within a clinical encounter create a context through which patients develop negative or positive expectations for treatments and clinical outcomes. Positively influencing patients’ beliefs about therapeutic success is one way to maximize placebo [2]. However, being too optimistic is also ethically problematic and can be construed as disingenuous if one is not cautious [7]. Manipulating a patient’s expectations may not necessarily require lying or deceiving. In studies of irritable bowel syndrome and migraine, patients were informed they were being treated with placebo and still developed a positive clinical response [21, 22]. But the emphatic and enthusiastic physicians’ presentation of clinical data in a patient certainly declares empathy from the physician’s site that in turn develops trust to the patient’s site, which consequently upsurges adherence and treatment outcome. Adherence is limited in conditions requiring chronic daily treatment that are associated with potential adverse events (AEs), in chronic pain disorders in particular [12]. Headache, the most common pain condition, remains one major health problem underestimated by all sites, patients, D. D. Mitsikostas (*) 1st Neurology Department, Aeginition Hospital, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_11 125 126 D. D. Mitsikostas families, doctors, media, and health-care decision-makers. Among several headache types, migraine and cluster headache [41] represent the most disabling primary headache disorders. Migraine in particular is rating as the second leading condition causing disability after low back pain worldwide, affecting 1.04 billion [17]. Tensiontype headache (TTH) is the most frequent headache disorder in the world with 1-year prevalence 38% [45] and 1.89 billion of people suffering from it (the third most prevalent condition among all medical ones) [17]. All headache disorders are treatable, but due to safety and tolerability reasons, available preventive treatments have often limited success, even in the right hands. Low adherence and comorbidity are key chains in the vicious cycle of chronic migraine therapeutics [19, 20, 49]. In this article the role of placebo and nocebo in headache therapeutics will be discussed with emphasis to techniques that improve the balance of placebo over nocebo. 11.2 Methodology to Estimate Nocebos in Clinical Practice While estimating nocebo response in controlled randomized clinical trials (RCTs) is easy, simply by referring to the AEs observed in the placebo-treated arm [1], in clinical practice it is almost impossible to estimate nocebo accurately and securely. The same stands for the placebo response. In RCTs two specific outcomes are used to assess nocebo: nocebo AEs (meaning the percentage of patients treated with placebo and experienced at least one AE) and nocebo dropouts (referring to the percentage of patients treated with placebo and discontinued treatment because of AE) [38]. But in clinical practice no such an assessment could be done. To overcome this pragmatic complexity, a questionnaire was evaluated to capture patients with potential future nocebo responses. This is a specific self-fulfilled questionnaire (Q-No), with 71.7% specificity, 67.5% sensitivity, and 42.5% positive predictive value for nocebo [32]. Q-No is a four-item (rating range 4–20) questionnaire addressing issues related to nocebo in outpatients seeking neurological consultation. When the total score is higher than 15 (Table 11.1), the physician should educate the patient for nocebo to minimize the potential patients’ negative expectations. There are Table 11.1 The Q-No questionnaire Question I read the summary of product characteristics (SPC) before taking a medication I have discontinued a medication because of adverse effects in the past I ask my physician for potential adverse effects of the medication he/she gives me I take into account the adverse effects reported in the summary of product characteristics (SPC) seriously Total score Rating Rating: 1 = never, 5 = always; by using a cutoff at score 15, the Q-No predicts nocebo with 71.7% specificity, 67.5% sensitivity, and 42.5% positive predictive value (Mitsikostas and Deligianni 2015 [32]) 11 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice 127 several limitations of the Q-No evaluation, however. Specificity, positive predictive value, and reliability are relatively low. Because participants in the evaluation of Q-No have had discussed the nocebo phenomenon with the treating neurologists, they were partially educated; thus nocebo responses may differ in this patients’ sample comparing to naive patients. Besides these limitations, however, Q-No may serve as a useful tool to predict potential nocebo responses in clinical practice. Capturing these patients and educating and treating them closely may limit nocebo and its obvious and severe consequences in outcome [32]. In a clinical observational study [33], 514 outpatients suffering from several headache disorders fulfilled the Q-No, and 291 (56.6%) scored more than 15 indicating potential nocebo behaviors. Post hoc analysis showed that those who were suffering from episodic forms of all three primary headache disorders (migraine, TTH, and cluster headache) displayed higher risk for nocebo behaviors compared to those who were suffering from chronic forms: two out of three patients suffering from episodic headaches scored more than 15 in Q-No compared to one out of two patients who were suffering from chronic subtypes. Migraineurs also showed higher risk for nocebo than those who were suffering from TTH (58.3% vs. 51.4%) [33]. In addition, nocebo influenced patients’ choices for the treatment. These data indicate that nocebo may worsen pain outcomes in an essential proportion of headache outpatients, raising the need for establishing techniques to boundary nocebo’s consequences in practicing, mainly by controlling the doctor-patient communication. 11.3 Placebo in Headache Treatment Even in an emotionally sterile patient-doctor communication, still the prototype placebo/nocebo behaviors do exist. In a sophisticated human experiment [21], the investigators tested the hypothesis that, in acute migraine, clinical outcomes with both placebo and medication treatment would increase monotonically as the pretreatment verbal information varies from negative (0% chance of receiving active medication) to uncertain (50% chance of medication) to positive (100% chance). They used migraine headache as a model because migraine is a naturally recurring neurological disorder of unilateral throbbing headache associated with variable incidence of aura, nausea, photophobia, allodynia, fatigue, and irritability. The recurring nature of migraine allowed the investigators to compare within each subject the efficacies of treatment and placebo over consecutive attacks using varying conditions of information. By manipulating the information provided to subjects, the primary analysis showed that the magnitude of headache relief induced by rizatriptan (a triptan for symptomatic migraine treatment), as well as placebo, was lowest when pills were labeled as placebo and higher when pills had uncertain labeling or were labeled as active medication. The effect was monotonic for placebo and nearly monotonic for rizatriptan. Interestingly, placebo treatment mislabeled as rizatriptan reduced headache severity as effectively as rizatriptan mislabeled as placebo, and open-label placebo treatment was superior to no treatment. Based on 128 D. D. Mitsikostas these findings, the authors concluded that (1) raising the likelihood of receiving active treatment for pain relief significantly contributes to increased success rate of triptan therapy for migraine, (2) open-label placebo treatment may have an important therapeutic benefit, and (3) placebo and medication effects can be modulated by expectancies [21]. As in migraine, a therapeutic benefit of open-label placebo vs. no treatment has also been reported for subjects with irritable bowel syndrome in a randomized controlled study [22] and in a pilot study in depression [23]. All together these data indicate that any single treatment contains placebo inevitably and in a reverse manner nocebo as well, which is in line with the hypothesis that both predispositions coexist in humans to favor perceptual mechanisms [9]. In practice, these perceptual mechanisms are triggered as soon as the physician suggests any treatment. But the kind of treatment and the way of suggestion along with several additional environmental and individual factors influence essentially the balance between these two opposite brain tendencies and control the final reaction. In clinical research for migraine, placebo-controlled studies are still the gold standard [13, 46], although the above data question this strategy. 11.4 Nocebo in Symptomatic Migraine Treatment Reuter and colleagues first investigated the AEs in patients treated with placebo in RCTs for the symptomatic treatment of migraine. In trials for symptomatic migraine treatment that tested the therapeutic efficacy of triptans, 21.9% of control patients reported at least one AE although treated with placebo. Symptoms were grouped into three categories: migraine-related (symptoms such as nausea, photophobia, and phonophobia), drug-related (symptoms typical of the experimental compound such as chest pressure in response to triptans), and nonspecific or coincidental (symptoms such as sleep disturbance). Thus, symptoms in the placebo group were related to the drug under study and to the symptomatology of migraine, whereas some others had no obvious relation to the condition or treatment [40]. In another review aimed at estimating the placebo response in migraineurs treated with oral triptans, it was found that 23.40–14.05% of participants treated with placebo reported AEs. Interestingly, studies performed in North America showed a higher nocebo frequency than those conducted in Europe [27]. Consequently, Amanzio and colleagues [1] published an extensive systematic review of nocebo in clinical trials for migraine. This was the first attempt to intensely investigate migraine-related nocebo effects. They investigated the AEs after placebo in RCTs testing NSAIDs, triptans, or anticonvulsants. Their major finding was that nocebo AEs mirrored the AEs expected of the active medication studied precisely. For example, anorexia and memory difficulties, which are typical AEs of anticonvulsants, were present only in the placebo arm of these trials. In other words, nocebo in migraine trials arose from patients’ distrust [1]. However, this important meta-analysis aimed to investigate mechanisms of nocebo in particular, rather than to investigate the magnitude of nocebo in RCTs for migraine. Migraine most likely was used as a vehicle pain 11 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice 129 condition in this study. For instance, the investigators searched RCTs for migraine trials, both symptomatic and preventive, only if specific anti-migraine agents were tested. Undoubtedly, the results of this meta-analysis confirmed their findings derived from experimental human studies that expectations modulate both nocebo and placebo (the expectation theory of placebo and nocebo) [1]. In another more recent meta-analysis of RCTs for all primary headache disorders, 56 RCTs published in the last decade were analyzed to estimate the frequency of patients treated with placebo who experience any AE (nocebo AE ratio) or discontinued treatment due to AE (nocebo dropout ratio) [36]. In this meta-analysis, all RCTs using any compound, either for acute or for chronic treatment, were included. The aim was to estimate the magnitude of nocebo in headaches in the most clinically relevant manner for both the clinicians and trial designers. In symptomatic treatments, nocebo dropout ratio was limited (0.33%), but in chronic preventive treatments was increased up to 5%, showing that 1 out of 20 patients treated for migraine prophylaxis discontinues treatment due to nocebo AEs. Practitioners should be aware of this fundamental nocebo effect, trial designers as well [30, 31, 36]. Stratified analyses in migraine studies revealed that: 1. Nocebo AEs and nocebo dropout ratios were higher in preventive trials than in symptomatic trials (P < 0.001). 2. Nocebo AE ratio varied by year of publication in trials for symptomatic treatment of migraine, decreasing from 22.05% (95% CI, 16.46%–28.21%) for trials published within 1998–2004 to 14.39% (95% CI, 10.81%–18.39%) for trials published within 2005–2009 (P < 0.001). 3. Nocebo may change with route administration (e.g. botulin toxin A). 4. No differences were found between studies performed in North America compared with Europe. 5. Dropout ratio was lower in the placebo group than in the active drug group (mean difference, 7.09%; 95% CI, 4.1%–10.1%; P < 0.0001). 6. Nocebo rates did not vary with the drug tested, with headache type, or by continent, with one exception (botulin toxin A) [36]. For symptomatic cluster headache treatment, four trials were analyzed in this meta-analysis, and the nocebo AE was estimated at 18.67% (95% CI 10.65–28.33%); insufficient data were gathered to calculate the nocebo dropouts in RCTs for cluster headache and tension-type headache [36]. 11.5 What Does Control Placebo and Nocebo? Several factors control placebo and nocebo, some of them being modifiable and some others not (Table 11.2). Previous experiences, personality, and a variety of cofactors modulate nocebo, even the price, the packing, or the tablet/capsule [37, 38, 48]. Pharmacophobia and nocebo are additional cofactors that border adherence and treatment outcomes significantly [30, 31]. Pharmacophobia refers to the 130 D. D. Mitsikostas Table 11.2 Factors influencing nocebo Modifiable Patient’s expectations Patient’s education Pre-treatment verbal suggestions Speed of treatment titration Safety profile of treatment Patient-doctor relation/communication Close follow-up Affective and cognitive traits Pharmacophobia Non-modifiable Previous negative experiences Personality Cultural factors Drug-related adverse events Social media and internet information Gender Treatment price Generic formulations Age The appearance of drugs or medical devices irrational fear of medication, and nocebo refers to the experience of adverse events related to patients’ negative expectation that a treatment will harm most likely instead of help [30]. The frequency of adverse events can dramatically increase by informing patients about the possible side effects of the treatment and by negative expectations on the part of the patient [4, 5]. Patients who were told that they might experience sexual side effects after treatment with β-blocker drugs reported these symptoms between three and four times more often than patients in a control group who were not informed about these symptoms [43]. Nocebo varies by the condition, and it has been estimated in several brain disorders such as migraine [1, 36], epilepsy [50, 51], fibromyalgia [34], multiple sclerosis [38], Parkinson’s disease [44], neuropathic pain [37], and restless syndrome [42] and in patients with depression [14, 29, 35]. Many other unpredictable factors like social media or Internet information cannot be detected. Instead an individual negative expectation originating from internal psychological structures could be predicted. A previous experience of drugrelated AEs and/or failed interventions are stable factors contributing to nocebo [8, 30, 31]. Whether these previously experienced AEs were or not related to nocebo may not change the risk for future nocebo responses. However, a negative patientdoctor communication facilitates nocebo [18]. Additionally, affective and cognitive traits could be important as well. Some personality traits and psychological factors, such as anxiety, harm avoidance and persistence, pessimism, and fear of pain, may influence the responsiveness to nocebo [3]. Finally, there is data indicating that treatment price, generic medicines [15, 16], and gender and age may influence nocebo and placebo responses in human studies, women being linked to conditioning rather than to expectancy and men the opposite, but this finding needs to be confirmed clinically [24, 47]. Children and adolescents show much higher placebo responses in headaches, up to 61% in studies for prevention of migraine [39], making treatments with proven efficacy in adults (e.g., topiramate and amitriptyline) unable to achieve a more effective clinical trial endpoint than placebo in the prevention of migraines in patients less than 18 years of age [25, 26]. There is no evidence for nocebo responses in children; but in trial with amitriptyline and topiramate [39], only 1.5% of patients treated with placebo discontinued treatment because of AEs vs. 5% in adults [36], indicating that children may display lower nocebo rates. By 11 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice 131 applying the Q-No in adult patients prior to consultation (e.g., in the waiting room), a physician treating headache patients can easily be informed for potential nocebo behaviors however and load specific and tailored approaches to limit them. 11.6 Persuading Patients Not only physicians but also nurses, other health-care professionals, and allied health services should be aware of their responsibility to avoid and reduce nocebo effects and the detrimental consequences of these effects, from diagnosis to therapy to prognosis [6]. The benefit of taking a medication (“this drug will reduce your migraine days from 8 you have now to 3 per month”) should always be included in patient information about newly prescribed drugs. Alternatively, a physician may explain the treatment benefit by using only positive outcomes, like “this drug will increase your headache-free days from 22 to 27 per month.” Drug treatment for primary headache disorders can be started at a very low dose (half- or one-quarter the recommended starting dose). The potential advantages of this approach include a further decrease in the risk of adverse events, enhanced patient participation, improved adherence, and reduced long-term costs [28]. “Start low, go slow” approach when starting a new medication also improves adherence in nocebo patients by minimizing the risk of AEs. Most importantly physicians should use an authentic and empathic communication style, providing adequate information regarding disease, diagnoses, treatments, and AEs. Close follow-up and discussing patients’ anxieties, concerns, and expectations are always required. Occasionally, it is very difficult to convince the patient, but it helps much when the physician explains the origin and the prevalence of nocebo. Informing the patient that he/she is not the only one who has this reaction (in headaches almost half of patients report nocebo AEs) and understanding the brain mechanisms underlying nocebo make the patient feel comfortable (Box 11.1) [6, 8, 18, 30, 31]. Box 11.1 Tips to Increase Placebo and Limit Nocebo • During the interview, be positive, authentic, and empathic; having face contact with the patient optimizes treatment expectation and expectation of adverse effects. Patients who have tried several alternative treatments before generally may dislike pharmaceutical interventions. • When prescribing a new medical treatment, use positive outcomes to explain their benefits (e.g., “this treatment will increase your headachefree days from 22 to 27 per month”), and encourage the patient not to Google for potential adverse events but to ask you. Go low and slow with the drug titration. Discuss the nocebo phenomenon. • Close follow-up to monitor potential adverse events (ask for a telephone follow-up a month after) or to reduce patient’s awareness. • Be tolerant, reachable, and persistent, in general. 132 11.7 D. D. Mitsikostas Conclusions Both placebo and nocebo are cognitive responses to all but therapies that coexist in humans and control outcomes radically. Physicians should acknowledge the mechanisms and factors controlling both reactions and plan individual strategies to change the balance of placebo in their practicing. Because personality powers both responses, physicians must certainly get the patient’s picture before decision-making and respect his/her principles. There are currently available several pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical treatment options in headaches that are being enriched substantially lately, enlarging the patients’ right of choice. One implication of respect for persons is a respect for personal autonomy, that is, acknowledging the moral right of every individual to choose and follow his or her own plan of life and actions. Given that one single adverse event does not cause the same implications among individuals nor patients share a common perception for it, doctors should describe the potential adverse events in their patients but in relation to treatment benefits. Treatment outcomes presented in a positive way affect patients significantly; thus a long interview may be required only for explaining the treatment benefits. Having in mind that even the worst headache case can be improved with a particular treatment, the physician should play a magic role to capture and convince suspicious patients who are not adhering, partly because previous physicians have not respected, or have not spent enough time to explore, their own individual choices for life. References 1. Amanzio M, Corazzini LL, Vase L, Benedetti F. A systematic review of adverse events in placebo groups of anti-migraine clinical trials. Pain. 2009;146(3):261–9. 2. Barefoot JC, Brummett BH, Williams RB, Siegler IC, Helms MJ, Clapp-Channing NE, Mark DB. Recovery expectations and long-term prognosis of patients with coronary heart disease. Arch Intern Med. 2011;171:929–35. 3. Bartels DJP, van Laarhoven AIM, Stroo M, Hijne K, Peerdeman KJ, Donders ART, van de Kerkhof PCM, Evers AWM. Minimizing nocebo effects by conditioning with verbal suggestion: a randomized clinical trial in healthy humans. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0182959. 4. Benedetti F, Amanzio M. The placebo response: how words and rituals change the patient’s brain. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;84(3):413–9. 5. Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When word are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience. 2007;147:260–71. 6. Bingel U. Avoiding Nocebo effects to optimize treatment outcome. JAMA. 2014;312:693–4. 7. Chavarria V, Vian J, Pereira C, Data-Franco J, Fernandes BS, Berk M, Dodd S. The placebo and Nocebo phenomena: their clinical management and impact on treatment outcomes. Clin Ther. 2017;39(3):477–86. 8. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA. 2012;307:567–8. 9. Colloca L, Miller FG. The nocebo effect and its relevance for clinical practice. Psychosom Med. 2011;73:598. 10. Colloca L. Nocebo effects can make you feel pain. Science. 2017;358:44. 11 Implications of Placebos and Nocebos in Clinical Practice 133 11. Colloca L. Placebo, nocebo, and learning mechanisms. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2014;225: 17–35. 12. Conn VS, Ruppar TM, Enriquez M, Cooper PS. Patient-centered outcomes of medication adherence interventions: systematic review and meta-analysis. Value Health. 2016;19(2):277–85. 13. Diener HC, Schorn CF, Bingel U, Dodick DW. The importance of placebo in headache research. Cephalalgia. 2008;28(10):1003–11. 14. Dodd S, Schacht A, Kelin K, Dueñas H, Reed VA, Williams LJ, Quirk FH, Malhi GS, Berk M. Nocebo effects in the treatment of major depression: results from an individual study participant-level meta-analysis of the placebo arm of duloxetine clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76(6):702–11. 15. Dunne S, Shannon B, Hannigan A, Dunne C, Cullen W. Physician and pharmacist perceptions of generic medicines: what they think and how they differ. Health Policy. 2014;116(2–3): 214–23. 16. Dunne SS, Shannon B, Cullen W, Dunne CP. Perceptions and attitudes of community pharmacists towards generic medicines. J Manag Care Spec Pham. 2014;20(11):1138–46. 17. GBD 2016 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. 2017;390:1211–59. 18. Greville-Harris M, Dieppe P. Bad is more powerful than good: the nocebo response in medical consultations. Am J Med. 2015;128:126–9. 19. Hepp Z, Bloudek LM, Varon SF. Systematic review of migraine prophylaxis adherence and persistence. J Manag Care Pharm. 2014;20:22–33. 20. Hepp Z, Dodick DW, Varon SF, Gillard P, Hansen RN, Devine EB. Adherence to oral migrainepreventive medications among patients with chronic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2015;35:478–88. 21. Kam-Hansen S, Jakubowski M, Kelley JM, Kirsch I, Hoaglin DC, Kaptchuk TJ, Burstein R. Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6(218):218ra5. 22. Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, Sanchez MN, Kokkotou E, Singer JP, Kowalczykowski M, Miller FG, Kirsch I, Lembo AJ. Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One. 2010;5:el5591. 23. Kelley JM, Kaptchuk TJ, Cusin C, Lipkin S, Fava M. Open-label placebo for major depressive disorder: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom. 2012;81(5):312–4. 24. Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Braun S, Kowalski A, Schrauth M, Zipfel S, Enck P. Gender and the nocebo response following conditioning and expectancy. J Psychosom Res. 2009;66(4):323–8. 25. Kroon Van Diest AM, Ernst MM, Slater S, Powers SW. Similarities and differences between migraine in children and adults: presentation, disability, and response to treatment. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2017;21(12):48. 26. Le K, Yu D, Wang J, Ali AI, Guo Y. Is topiramate effective for migraine prevention in patients less than 18 years of age? A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Headache Pain. 2017;18(1):69. 27. Loder E, Goldstein R, Biondi D. Placebo effects in oral triptan trials: the scientific and ethical rationale for continued use of placebo controls. Cephalalgia. 2005;25:124–31. 28. McCormack JP, GM MD, Virani AS. Is bigger better? An argument for very low starting doses. CMAJ. 2011;183:65–9. 29. Meister R, Jansen A, Härter M, Nestoriuc Y, Kriston L. Placebo and nocebo reactions in randomized trials of pharmacological treatments for persistent depressive disorder. A metaregression analysis. J Affect Disord. 2017;215:288–98. 30. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headache. Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;29(3):331–6. 31. Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in headaches: implications for clinical practice and trial design. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2012;12:132–7. 32. Mitsikostas DD, Deligianni CI. Q-no: a questionnaire to predict nocebo in & outpatients seeking neurological consultation. Neurol Sci. 2015;36:379–81. 134 D. D. Mitsikostas 33. Mitsikostas DD, Belesioti I, Arvaniti C, Mitropoulou E, Deligianni C, Kasioti E, Constantinidis T, Dermitzakis M, Vikelis M. Hellenic headache society. Patients’ preferences for headache acute and preventive treatment. J Headache Pain. 2017;18(1):102. 34. Mitsikostas DD, Chalarakis NG, Mantonakis LI, Delicha EM, Sfikakis PP. Nocebo in fibromyalgia: meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials and implications for practice. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(5):672–80. 35. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis L, Chalarakis N. Nocebo in clinical trials for depression: a metaanalysis. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(1):82–6. 36. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis LI, Chalarakis NG. Nocebo is the enemy, not placebo. A meta-analysis of reported side effects after placebo treatment in headaches. Cephalalgia. 2011;31:550–61. 37. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. A meta-analytic approach to estimating nocebo effects in neuropathic pain trials. J Neurol. 2012;259:436–47. 38. Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effects in multiple sclerosis trials: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler. 2010;16:816–28. 39. Powers SW, Coffey CS, Chamberlin LA, Ecklund DJ, Klingner EA, Yankey JW, Korbee LL, Porter LL, Hershey AD, CHAMP Investigators. Trial of amitriptyline, topiramate, and placebo for pediatric migraine. N Engl J Med. 2017;376(2):115–24. 40. Reuter U, Sanchez del Rio M, Carpay JA, Boes CJ, Silberstein SD. GSK headache masters program: placebo adverse events in headache trials: headache as an adverse event of placebo. Cephalalgia. 2003;23:496–503. 41. Rozen TD, Fishman RS. Cluster headache in the United States of America: demographics, clinical characteristics, triggers, suicidality, and personal burden. Headache. 2012;52:99–113. 42. Silva MA, Duarte GS, Camara R, Rodrigues FB, Fernandes RM, Abreu D, Mestre T, Costa J, Trenkwalder C, Ferreira JJ. Placebo and nocebo responses in restless legs syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2017;88(23):2216–24. 43. Silvestri A, Galetta P, Cerquetani E, Marazzi G, Patrizi R, Fini M, Rosano GM. Report of erectile dysfunction after therapy with beta-blockers is related to patient knowledge of side effects and is reversed by placebo. Eur Heart J. 2003;24(21):1928–32. 44. Stathis P, Smpiliris M, Konitsiotis S, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo as a potential confounding factor in clinical trials for Parkinson's disease treatment: a meta-analysis. Eur J Neurol. 2013;20:527–33. 45. Steiner TJ, Stovner LJ, Katsarava Z, Lainez JM, Lampl C, Lantéri-Minet M, Rastenyte D, Ruiz de la Torre E, Tassorelli C, Barré J, Andrée C. The impact of headache in Europe: principal results of the Eurolight project. J Headache Pain. 2014;15:31. 46. Thorlund K, Toor K, Wu P, Chan K, Druyts E, Ramos E, Bhambri R, Donnet A, Stark R, Goadsby PJ. Comparative tolerability of treatments for acute migraine: a network meta-analysis. Cephalalgia. 2017;37(10):965–78. 47. Tinnermann A, Geuter S, Sprenger C, Finsterbusch J, Büchel C. Interactions between brain and spinal cord mediate value effects in nocebo hyperalgesia. Science. 2017;358(6359):105–8. 48. Webster RK, Weinman J, Rubin GJ. A systematic review of factors that contribute to nocebo effects. Health Psychol. 2016;35(12):1334–55. 49. Woolley JM, Bonafede MM, Maiese BA, Lenz RA. Migraine prophylaxis and acute treatment patterns among commercially insured patients in the United States. Headache. 2017;57(9):1399–408. 50. Zaccara G, Giovannelli F, Giorgi FS, Franco V, Gasparini S. Analysis of nocebo effects of antiepileptic drugs across different conditions. J Neurol. 2016;263(7):1274–9. 51. Zis P, Shafiq F, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effect in refractory partial epilepsy during pre-surgical monitoring: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. Seizure. 2017;45:95–9. Chapter 12 Informed Consent and the Ethics of Placebo-Based Interventions in Clinical Practice Marco Annoni and Franklin G. Miller 12.1 Introduction In recent years a converging series of empirical studies has begun to unravel the psychobiological mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects [13]. These studies have demonstrated that placebo effects may significantly modulate a plethora of symptoms in highly prevalent conditions such as pain, depression, irritable bowel syndrome, and migraine [5, 19]. For instance, in an experiment by Kham-Hansen et al., patients with recurring migraine were randomized using a 2 × 3 balanced placebo design to receive either a placebo or an effective medication (Maxalt) under three information conditions: truthful disclosure (“Maxalt” or “placebo”), blinded treatment (50% chance of receiving Maxalt or placebo), and deceptive disclosure (told “Maxalt” but given placebo; told “placebo” but given Maxalt) [17]. The study demonstrated that Maxalt was more efficacious than placebo across all information conditions. However, the study also found that Maxalt mislabeled “placebo” was nearly as effective as a placebo mislabeled “Maxalt” and that the effect of a truthful placebo was superior to no treatment. Remarkably, the placebo effect “was significant under each labeling condition relative to no treatment, amounting in magnitude to >50% of Maxalt effect under the corresponding labeling condition” [17]. These findings suggest not only that placebo effects may enhance the therapeutic benefits of both placebos and proven therapies but also that different information disclosures may elicit different magnitudes of placebo effects, presumably through the modulation of patients’ expectations. M. Annoni (*) Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Istituto di Tecnologie Biomediche, Rome, Italy e-mail: [email protected] F. G. Miller Weill Cornell Medical College, New York, NY, USA e-mail: [email protected] © Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019 D. D. Mitsikostas, F. Benedetti (eds.), Placebos and Nocebos in Headaches, Headache, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02976-0_12 135 136 M. Annoni and F. G. Miller Other treatments for migraine and other forms of headache have been found to have similarly strong placebo components, most notably acupuncture. A 2016 Cochrane review sampled 22 trials with 4985 participants in total to ascertain whether acupuncture was more effective than no treatment, sham acupuncture, and prophylactic treatment to reduce migraine frequency [21]. This comprehensive study found that acupuncture was associated with a moderate reduction of migraine frequency over no acupuncture and with a small but statistically significant reduction in frequency over sham after treatment and at follow-up [21]. Acupuncture reduced migraine frequency significantly more than drug prophylaxis after treatment, and patients receiving acupuncture were less likely to report adverse side effects and to discontinue participation in trials as compared with patients receiving conventional treatments. Based on these results, this review concluded that acupuncture may be “considered a treatment option” for migraine patients, even though most of its benefits may be due to placebo effects. Collectively, these and other studies suggest that placebo effects may account for a significant part of the benefits of conventional and complementary treatments currently prescribed to treat or prevent migraine. These empirical findings, however, raise a host of ethical issues regarding whether and how clinicians could ethically promote placebo effects and minimize nocebo effects with the aim of maximizing therapeutic benefits without compromising informed consent [24]. In particular, it is possible to distinguish three ways in which clinicians may mobilize placebo effects for therapeutic purposes, each raising a different set of moral considerations. First, clinicians may resort to paternalistic deception—for instance, recommending or administering a placebo as if it were an effective medication (e.g., “Maxalt”). According to one view in medical ethics, paternalistic deception may sometimes be morally permissible, but only when its prospective benefits outweigh the potential risks, the infringement of patient’s autonomy, and the threatening of the bond of trust [1, 4, 20]. Second, clinicians may attempt to prescribe placebos without deception—i.e., “open-label placebos.” In principle, this option sidesteps the ethical hurdles of using deception. However, the evidence supporting the efficacy of open-label placebos is still limited to a few pilot studies [28]. Furthermore, there is an ongoing debate about whether and how an open-label placebo can be transparently prescribed without equivocation [3, 6, 16]. While these scenarios have been already analyzed in some depth elsewhere, in this chapter we focus on a third category of morally relevant and less explored cases: the one regarding the prescription of treatments that, like acupuncture, has been proven to be slightly more effective than placebos and yet significantly better than no treatment or the standard or care—a class of remedies that we shall henceforth label as “placebo-based interventions.” 12.2 The Ethics of Prescribing Placebo-Based Interventions Under which conditions could doctors prescribe placebo-based interventions? In order to answer this preliminary question, let us consider again the case of recurring migraine—a highly prevalent, painful, and potentially disabling condition. While 12 Informed Consent and the Ethics of Placebo-Based Interventions in Clinical Practice 137 there are several proven pharmacological and non-pharmacological therapies for treating and preventing migraine, in each case the prescription may vary considerably depending on patients’ preferences and clinical features of the attacks [23]. Moreover, some individuals with recurring migraine are refractory to guideline-based treatment, while others cannot be treated with pharmacological remedies due to comorbidities, the severity of side effects, or the risk of medication overuse [22, 29]. Thus, there are conceivable cases in which, based on the available evidence, acupuncture may be the only, or the most, effective clinical option for a patient. In these circumstances, we argue, there is no reason why doctors should not consider the prescription of an effective treatment, even though most of its benefits are due to placebo effects, as long as (1) no other comparable clinical option is available; (2) no deception is involved; (3) the benefits outweigh the risks; (4) the costs are proportioned to the benefits; and (5) the treatment is in line with patients’ needs. (By extension, this argument can also be generalized to the prescription of other placebo-based interventions in other conditions prone to placebo modulation, such as pain or irritable bowel syndrome.) However, while these threshold conditions apply to all prescriptions, intentionally recommending a placebo-based intervention requires additional caution for three reasons. First, despite recent scientific advancements in the characterization of their underlying mechanisms, placebo and nocebo effects remain elusive phenomena [5, 26, 27]. Placebo effects may vary considerably from one context to another and across individuals [15]. Hence, it is difficult to anticipate the magnitude of the benefit that placebo-based treatments might have in each case. Second, some placebo-based interventions may be contrary to patients’ preferences and beliefs. For instance, patients may be skeptical or opposed to complementary and alternative remedies. Hence, it is crucial to probe in advance patient’s attitudes toward the recommended placebo-based intervention. Additionally, this might allow patients to better match their preferences and beliefs with available therapeutic options, enhancing their sense of control—a factor that has been shown to positively contribute to the modulation of clinical outcomes [14]. Finally, it is important for physicians to be more aware and educated about placebo and nocebo effects [7]. As several empirical studies have demonstrated, the quality of the therapeutic interaction and the features of the clinician delivering the therapy may all be important variables of placebo modulation [5, 21]. Being aware of such determinants of placebo effects might lead physicians prescribing a placebo-based treatment such as acupuncture to recommend a particular practitioner, thus indirectly enhancing the placebo component of the proposed therapy. 12.3 lacebo-Based Interventions and the Ethics of Informed P Consent Provided the former conditions are met, the next relevant ethical issue is how doctors should communicate when recommending placebo-based interventions. In particular, should doctors disclose that most of the benefits of the prescribed treatment may be due to placebo effects? 138 M. Annoni and F. G. Miller This question suggests an ethical dilemma. On the one hand, clinicians have a duty of beneficence. This moral obligation entails that doctors must act to serve patients’ best interests. The primary purpose of prescribing a placebo-based intervention is to alleviate patients’ symptoms mostly through beneficial placebo effects. However, as evidence suggests, the strength of patients’ expectations may correlate with the magnitude of the placebo effects [5]. Disclosing that the prescribed treatment is “placebo-based” might hinder patients’ expectations, thus leading to weaker placebo effects and suboptimal clinical outcomes. On the other hand, clinicians have a duty of respecting patient’s autonomy and right to informed consent. This moral obligation entails that clinicians have a prima facie duty of veracity in all their professional communications: other things being equal, doctors must be honest and tell the truth to patients. The duty of veracity, in turn, can be further understood as the conjunction of two more specific duties: the negative duty not to lie and deceive and the positive duty to provide all the necessary information to respect and foster patients’ autonomous agency [2, 4]. For simplicity, let us call the former the duty of truthfulness and the latter the duty to inform. Clearly, a physician introducing acupuncture by saying (a) “this treatment has been proven effective to reduce the frequency of migraine attacks” would not violate the duty of truthfulness, as this statement is factually true. It is, however, less clear whether this statement alone violates the duty to inform or whether the clinician should add (b) “…although most of its benefits may be due to placebo effects.” In our view, while deciding in such scenarios, doctors should adopt a pragmatic criterion and add sentence (b) only if it would make a conceivable difference for patient’s decisional autonomy; otherwise they can omit it out of consideration of beneficence. Consider, for example, the case of a patient suffering from recurring migraine who is refractory to pharmacological treatments. For this patient acupuncture may represent the only clinical option to reduce the frequency of future disabling attacks. If the doctor concludes, after probing patient’s views and preferences, that he would be willing to try any treatment that could help, then he might introduce acupuncture using only statement (a). In this case, we argue, adding statement (b) would likely not enhance patient’s autonomy in any significant way, and yet it will possibly diminish the benefits of undergoing the treatment. In this case, since (a) is a truthful statement, and (b) does not obviously fall under the duty to inform, a doctor introducing acupuncture using only (a) would not violate his general duty of veracity. After all, the benefits of many effective treatments—from analgesics to antidepressants—are thought to be due to pharmacological and placebo effects, and yet ­doctors have no duty of informing patients while prescribing these treatments that the final outcome may in part be due to a non-pharmacological cause. By contrast, consider the case of a doctor who ponders chiropractic spinal manipulative therapy (CSMT) as a treatment for a patient in the same conditions. This doctor is aware of a recent three-armed, single-blinded, placebo, randomized controlled trial in which the efficacy of CSMT has been evaluated versus placebo (“sham chiropractic”) and control (usual pharmacological management) [8]. (“Sham 12 Informed Consent and the Ethics of Placebo-Based Interventions in Clinical Practice 139 chiropractic” was a sham push maneuver that had been validated by a previous study as a legitimate placebo control [9].) This study found that migraine frequency was significantly reduced in all three groups from baseline to posttreatment. However, while the effects persisted in both the CSMT and the placebo group at follow-up, they returned to baseline in the control group. With respect to all endpoints, no significant difference was found between CSMT and placebo. Hence, this study suggests that CSMT is an “effective” treatment for migraine, although only because of placebo effects, and that both CSMT and sham chiropractic may be superior to usual pharmacological management over the medium term. Thus, differently from the case of acupuncture, and assuming these empirical findings to be correct, CSMT has no specific efficacy for migraine and operates only through placebo effects. Importantly, this difference impacts how doctors should introduce CSMT in order to achieve a valid, nondeceptive, informed consent. In fact, if a doctor introduces CSMT by saying (c) “this treatment has been proven effective to reduce the frequency of migraine attacks,” then (c) could qualify as a deceptive statement if it is meant to instill in the patient the false belief that CSMT is “effective” in the same sense in which therapies like Maxalt are “effective”—i.e., superior to placebo. Yet, the term “effective” is slightly ambiguous, as it might be intended in a more ordinary sense as meaning that a treatment is “beneficial and superior to no treatment.” In this latter sense, (c) would arguably be true, and thus it would not formally violate the duty of truthfulness. However, even if we concede that the term “effective” may be intended in the latter sense, introducing CSMT without adding that its benefits may be (entirely) due to placebo effects would still violate clinicians’ duty to inform. This is demonstrated by the fact that (c) could likewise be used to prescribe/recommend placebo pills or sham acupuncture, as both have been demonstrated to be “effective”—i.e., superior to no treatment—in controlled studies. Yet, intentionally prescribing/recommending a placebo treatment without patient’s informed consent represents a clear failure to uphold clinicians’ duty of respecting patient’s autonomy. If the patient later discovers that the recommended treatment (a pill, acupuncture, or chiropractic) was “just a placebo,” legitimately, he may feel duped and manipulated by the doctor in a way that is incompatible with the respect of his decisional autonomy. Furthermore, this may also lead to harmful consequences for the therapeutic relationship and the bond of trust. Of course, one could argue that, given the expected benefits, sometimes it might be ethical to prescribe deceptively a placebo-based treatment—or even a placebo treatment—in violation of the duty of veracity. It is important, however, to underscore that, in this case, the doctor would resort to paternalistic deception, a practice that is incompatible with the provision of a valid informed consent and that might be justifiable only in exceptional cases [2, 18]. In general, the acupuncture and CSMT examples illustrate how complex it is for doctors to achieve a valid informed consent and respect patient’s autonomy when the recommended treatment has a dominant or exclusive placebo component. A final issue concerns the possible harm that informed consent may cause due to nocebo effects. Clinicians’ duty to inform covers also the possible side effects 140 M. Annoni and F. G. Miller of prescribed therapies and procedures. Some of these side effects may be amenable to nocebo modulation, such as headache or insomnia [25]. In disclosing these possible effects, clinicians may inadvertently increase the likelihood of patients experiencing nocebo-related side effects. With respect to this issue, scholars have suggested three strategies. The first strategy is to override the duty to inform, considering “nondisclosure in the case when a nocebo effect is likely to occur” [12]. This solution, however, may be appropriate only in those rare cases in which the possible harm of nocebo effects is so severe as to justify a breach of clinicians’ duty of veracity [10, 11]. A second strategy, then, is that of minimizing nocebo effects without forestalling the duty of veracity. This can be accomplished by informing patients about nocebo effects, and offering them the possibility of voluntarily waiving to receive side-effect information [25], and by using framing effects to reduce possible nocebo side effects [12]. Clinicians may resort to this second strategy in those cases in which the prescription of a placebo-based intervention includes the disclosure of information that may cause mild and reversible nocebo effects. Finally, a third strategy is to educate patients (and clinicians) more about nocebo effects, so as to help them “understand and contextualize the potential for anxiety and other psychophysiological mechanisms that could lead to the occurrence of unwanted nocebo effects” [10]. 12.4 Conclusion Placebo effects may modulate clinical outcomes in many symptomatic conditions, including headache and migraine. However, the intentional manipulation of placebo effects in clinical settings poses the ethical question of how clinicians ought to communicate with patients in order to maximize their therapeutic benefits while at the same time securing a valid informed consent. This ethical issue is particularly thorny when it regards the prescription of placebo-based treatments, i.e., of remedies that have been found slightly more effective than placebos in controlled experiments and yet better than no treatment or the standard of care. With respect to this kind of treatment, in this chapter we have argued that doctors may ethically prescribe placebo-based interventions in nondeceptive ways, provided that no other comparable clinical option is available; the benefits outweigh the risks; the costs are proportional; and the treatment is in line with patients’ needs, values, and preferences. Finally, we have contended that, while prescribing placebo-based interventions, doctors may ethically omit to mention that most of the benefits of the prescribed treatment are due to placebo effects out of considerations of beneficence, but only in those cases in which the treatment has been proven more effective than a comparable placebo, and this information would not make a conceivable difference for patients’ autonomy. 12 Informed Consent and the Ethics of Placebo-Based Interventions in Clinical Practice 141 References 1. Annoni M, Miller FG. Placebo in clinical practice: an ethical overview. Douleur Analg. 2014;27:215–20. 2. Annoni M. Exceptional lies: the ethics of deceptive placebos in clinical settings. Biblioteca della libertà. 2015;213:1–17. 3. Annoni M, Miller FG. Placebo effects and the ethics of therapeutic communication: a pragmatic perspective. Kennedy Inst Ethics J. 2016;26:79–103. 4. Beauchamp TL, Childress JF. Principles of biomedical ethics. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. 5. Benedetti F. Placebo effects: understanding the mechanisms in health and disease. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015. 6. Blease C, Colloca L, Kaptchuk TJ. Are open-label placebos ethical? Informed consent and ethical considerations. Bioethics. 2016;30:407–14. 7. Blease CR, Bishop FL, Kaptchuk TJ. Informed consent and clinical trials: where is the placebo effect? BMJ. 2017;356:j463. 8. Chaibi A, Benth JS, Russell MB. Validation of placebo in a manual therapy randomized controlled trial. Sci Rep. 2015;5:11774. 9. Chaibi A, Benth JS, Tuchin PJ, Russell MB. Chiropractic spinal manipulative therapy for migraine: a three-armed, single-blinded, placebo, randomized controlled trial. Eur J Neurol. 2017;24:143–53. 10. Colloca L. Tell me the truth and I will not be harmed: informed consents and nocebo effects. Am J Bioeth. 2017;17:46–8. 11. Colloca L, Finniss D. Nocebo effects, patient-clinician communication, and therapeutic outcomes. JAMA. 2012;307:567–8. 12. Fortunato J, Wasserman J, Menkes DL. When respecting autonomy is harmful: a clinically useful approach to the nocebo effect. Am J Bioeth. 2017;17:36–42. 13. Hall KT, Lembo AJ, Kirsch I, Ziogas DC, Douaiher J, Jensen KB, Conboy LA, Kelley JM, Kokkotou E, Kaptchuk TJ. Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met polymorphism predicts placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One. 2012;7:e48135. 14. Justman S. Deceit and transparency in placebo research. Yale J Biol Med. 2013;86:323–31. 15. Kam-Hansen S, Jakubowski M, Kelley JM, Kirsch I, Hoaglin DC, Kaptchuk TJ, Burstein R. Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci Transl Med. 2014;6:218ra215. 16. Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, Davis RB, Kerr CE, Jacobson EE, et al. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ. 2008;336:999. 17. Kaptchuk TJ, Miller FG. Placebo effects in medicine. N Engl J Med. 2015;373:8–9. 18. Kolber AJ. A limited defense of clinical placebo deception. Yale Policy Rev. 2007;26:75–134. 19. Linde K, Allais G, Brinkhaus B, Fei Y, Mehring M, Vertosick EA, Vickers A, White AR. Acupuncture for the prevention of episodic migraine. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;6:CD001218. 20. Lionetto L, Negro A, Palmisani S, et al. Emerging treatment for chronic migraine and refractory chronic migraine. Expert Opin Emerg Drugs. 2012;17:393–406. 21. Martelletti P. The therapeutic armamentarium in migraine is quite elderly. Expert Opin Drug Metab Toxicol. 2015;11:175–7. 22. Miller FG, Colloca L. The legitimacy of placebo treatments in clinical practice: evidence and ethics. Am J Bioeth. 2009;9:39–47. 23. Miller FG, Colloca L, Kaptchuk TJ. The placebo effect: illness and interpersonal healing. Perspect Biol Med. 2009;52:518–39. 24. Miller FG, Brody H. Understanding and harnessing placebo effects: clearing away the underbrush. J Med Philos. 2011;36:69–78. 142 M. Annoni and F. G. Miller 25. Miller FG, Colloca L. The placebo phenomenon and medical ethics: rethinking the relationship between informed consent and risk-benefit assessment. Theor Med Bioeth. 2011;32:229–43. 26. Miller FG. The concept and significance of the placebo effect. In: Miller FG, Colloca L, Crouch R, Kaptchuk TJ, editors. The placebo: a reader. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press; 2013. p. 1–9. 27. Negro A, Martelletti P. Chronic migraine plus medication overuse headache: two entities or not? J Headache Pain. 2011;12:593–601. 28. Petkovic G, Charlesworth JEG, Kelley J, et al. Effects of placebos without deception compared with no treatment: protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e009428. 29. Wager TD, Atlas L. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:403–17.